* A Distributed Proofreaders Canada eBook *

This eBook is made available at no cost and with very few restrictions. These restrictions apply only if (1) you make a change in the eBook (other than alteration for different display devices), or (2) you are making commercial use of the eBook. If either of these conditions applies, please contact a https://www.fadedpage.com administrator before proceeding. Thousands more FREE eBooks are available at https://www.fadedpage.com.

This work is in the Canadian public domain, but may be under copyright in some countries. If you live outside Canada, check your country's copyright laws. IF THE BOOK IS UNDER COPYRIGHT IN YOUR COUNTRY, DO NOT DOWNLOAD OR REDISTRIBUTE THIS FILE.

Title: The Historical Study of the Mother Tongue

Date of first publication: 1906

Author: Henry Cecil Wyld (1870-1945)

Date first posted: June 10, 2023

Date last updated: June 10, 2023

Faded Page eBook #20230614

This eBook was produced by: Marcia Brooks, Al Haines, Howard Ross & the online Distributed Proofreaders Canada team at https://www.pgdpcanada.net



BY THE SAME AUTHOR
A SHORT HISTORY OF ENGLISH. With a Bibliography of Recent Books on the Subject, and Lists of Texts and Editions.
STUDIES IN ENGLISH RHYMES FROM SURREY TO POPE. A Chapter in the History of English.
THE TEACHING OF READING IN TRAINING COLLEGES.
THE PLACE OF THE MOTHER TONGUE IN NATIONAL EDUCATION. Paper covers.
THE GROWTH OF ENGLISH. An Elementary Account of the Present Form of our Language, and its Development.

[All rights reserved]


THE HISTORICAL STUDY OF

THE MOTHER TONGUE

 

AN INTRODUCTION TO PHILOLOGICAL

METHOD

 

BY HENRY CECIL WYLD

 

MERTON PROFESSOR OF THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE IN THE UNIVERSITY OF OXFORD

 

 

LONDON

JOHN MURRAY, ALBEMARLE STREET, W.

Please see: Transcriber’s Notes

First EditionNovember, 1906
Reprinted November, 1907
Reprinted November, 1918
Reprinted October, 1920
Reprinted April, 1926

PRINTED IN GREAT BRITAIN BY

BILLING AND SONS, LTD., GUILDFORD AND ESHER


PREFACE

In undertaking the task of writing such a work as the present small volume, I did not disguise from myself the difficulty of what lay before me; now that I have completed it, I am in no way blind to the imperfections of the achievement. In a sense, the object of the book is a modest one—to give, not the history of our language, but some indications of the point of view from which the history of a language should be studied, and of the principal points of method in such a study. These methods are chiefly determined by the views which are held at the present time concerning the nature of language, and the mode of its development; and such views, in their turn, are based upon the knowledge of facts, concerning the life-history of many languages, which have been patiently accumulated during the last eighty years. I have hoped, in the following pages, to prepare the way for the beginner, to the study of at least some of the great writers who have been the pioneers of our knowledge of the development of our own tongue, and of its relations to other languages, as well as the chief framers of contemporary philological theory. Thus the present work aims at no more than to serve as an introduction to the more advanced scientific study of linguistic problems in the pages of first-hand authorities.

Advanced text-books of the German type are naturally almost unintelligible to the beginner, who has not undergone some preliminary training in philological aim and method. Of the text-books published in this country, which are nearly all of a more popular description, some are—to our shame be it spoken—mere cram-books, which strive only to give such ‘tips’ as shall enable the reader to pass certain examinations, while several others, by writers of repute and learning, are lacking in any general statement of principles or reference to authorities, in case the student should by chance wish to pursue the subject further than the covers of this or that small if admirable book. Again, a serious defect, as it appears to me, of many of the best elementary books on the History of English, is that the bare facts are stated, dogmatically and categorically, without any suggestion as to the sources of information or the methods of arriving at the results stated. As a practical teacher of English to University students of various stages of knowledge, from beginners onwards, I know that intelligent students are often irritated, on the one hand, by not being told how certain facts concerning past forms of speech are arrived at, and, on the other hand, by finding no reference to authorities who might give them the information which the writer of the manual so often withholds.

The worst feature in the withholding of such information is that the solitary student, who has not access to University classes, after he has read the books and mastered the facts, has yet not received anything in the shape of a training in the actual methods of the science of language; he has acquired a knowledge of a certain number of facts. but they exist in his mind isolated, and unrelated to anything else, least of all to a principle of wide application. Thus he acquires no new outlook upon linguistic phenomena, no method whereby he can pursue the subject for himself. It is believed that the chapters upon General Principles which follow, may be of use in putting the student upon right lines of further thought and study.

In dealing with general questions, I have sought as far as possible to illustrate principles by concrete examples drawn from the development of English.

In treating the more specific problems connected with the Aryan and Germanic languages I have sought, not so much to supplement the knowledge which it is possible to derive from the usual small work on Comparative Philology, as to make this clear on those points where I have found uncertainty to exist in the minds of students as to the precise bearing of this or that statement, and also to relate this part of the subject to general principles of the history of language on one hand, and on the other to the history of our own language. I thought it advisable to add a chapter on Methods of Reconstruction, since, although most of the small text-books teem with references to Parent Aryan, I have never yet found a student who had gathered from their pages how anyone knew what Parent Aryan was like. In this section, as throughout the book, I have striven to keep ever before the mind of the student the fact that we are dealing with changes in actual speech sounds, and not with letters, which is, unfortunately, too often the impression gathered from elementary manuals. I believed that a brief statement concerning the phenomena grouped together under the name Ablaut or Gradation would be useful, seeing that any explanation of them is generally omitted in the kind of books referred to—even in the best.

The task of selection, in treating the development of English itself, was very difficult, and I do not claim to have accomplished it with perfect success. Among the books generally accessible to students who are compelled to tackle the subject without the help of an experienced and highly trained teacher, there are several which contain an admirable marshalling of facts. Since I believed it desirable to devote a large portion of so small a book as the present to general questions, space was not available to restate facts which are to be found in most other books corresponding in size to the present volume. I therefore tried to select such points as I have found are generally the least well understood by ordinary students with no special training, but which are, nevertheless, of the greatest importance to a proper understanding of the facts of present-day English. I have tried, amongst other things, to emphasize, rather more than is usually the case in books for beginners, the rise of double forms in Middle English, and to show how often both doublets survive, if not in standard English, then in the modern dialects—one type in this form of present-day English, another in that. It is desirable that students should realize that much that is considered ‘vulgar’ in English is merely so by convention—for the reason, that is, that the polite dialect has selected another form, but that a very large number of ‘vulgarisms’ are historically quite as ‘correct’ as the received form. This knowledge must tend to a saner and a more scientific view of what is ‘right’ or ‘wrong’ in speech. My debts to other books of various kinds are, it need hardly be said, innumerable. I trust that I have made some, if not adequate, acknowledgment in the references given hereafter.

I am proud to acknowledge a special debt to Dr. Henry Sweet, one that is far deeper than any I could have contracted by the mere use of his books, great as that is. For many years past, the cordial personal intercourse which I have been privileged to enjoy with Dr. Sweet, has been an unfailing source of stimulus and enlightenment. I regret that this little work is not a worthier tribute to his teaching and influence. If the following pages should contribute at all to a wider adoption of Dr. Sweet’s Phonetic and Historical Methods, in Training Colleges and in the upper forms of secondary schools, and among private students, it will help to bring about a sounder mode of study of our own tongue than that which is commonly pursued in the majority of such institutions.

It is a pleasant duty to express my gratitude to Miss Irene F. Williams, M.A., formerly Research Fellow of the University of Liverpool, who most generously undertook the laborious task of compiling the index to the present volume. This contribution, by an expert English philologist, must, I feel sure, materially increase the utility of the book.

HENRY CECIL WYLD.

Alvescot, Oxon,

   July, 1906.

CONTENTS

CHAPTERPAGE
IINTRODUCTION; THE AIMS OF HISTORICAL LINGUISTIC STUDY1
IITHE SOUNDS OF SPEECH27
IIIHOW LANGUAGE IS ACQUIRED AND HANDED ON55
IVSOUND CHANGE67
VDIFFERENTIATION OF LANGUAGE: THE RISE OF DIALECTS91
VILINGUISTIC CONTACT119
VIIANALOGY128
VIIIMETHODS OF COMPARISON AND RECONSTRUCTION141
IXTHE ARYAN OR INDO-GERMANIC MOTHER-TONGUE, AND THE DERIVED FAMILIES OF LANGUAGES165
XTHE GERMANIC FAMILY195
XITHE HISTORY OF ENGLISH: GENERAL REMARKS ON THE SCOPE AND NATURE OF THE INQUIRY, AND THE MAIN PROBLEMS CONNECTED WITH IT205
XIIHISTORY OF ENGLISH: THE OLD ENGLISH PERIOD216
XIIITHE MIDDLE ENGLISH PERIOD250
XIVCHANGES IN ENGLISH PRONUNCIATION DURING THE MODERN PERIOD—THE DEVELOPMENT OF ENGLISH SOUNDS FROM THE FIFTEENTH CENTURY TO THE PRESENT DAY299
XVTHE STUDY OF PRESENT-DAY ENGLISH339
SUBJECT INDEX382
WORD INDEX393
LIST OF AUTHORITIES REFERRED TO409

CHAPTER I

INTRODUCTION; THE AIMS OF HISTORICAL LINGUISTIC STUDY

The practical study of language, or rather the study of language for practical purposes, is familiar to everyone, and plays, of necessity, a large part in all schemes of education. In infancy and childhood the mother-tongue is gradually, although instinctively, acquired. Later on, the native tongue becomes the subject of more deliberate study, and to this is added, for the most part, that of other languages, both living and dead.

It is convenient to consider as ‘practical’ that study of languages which has as its aim the mastery of tongues for the purpose of using them—that is, for the purpose either of speaking or reading them, or both.

From this point of view the schoolboy acquires, with various degrees of success, the pronunciation, the vocabulary, and the general structure of several languages, both ancient and modern. He is instructed in the rules of inflection and of syntax; he masters many exceptions, which perhaps, in his eyes, hardly serve to prove the rule.

In all this study of Latin and Greek, English, French, and German, which in this country occupies the chief energies of boyhood and early manhood, the view of language which is perpetually before the mind of the student is one and the same—namely, that of language in a state of suspended animation, stationary, and unchanging. That is to say, that the various languages are studied merely in the forms in which they exist at a particular period of their development. There is, as a rule, but little suggestion from the teacher that the language under consideration has developed from something very different; still less that, if it is a living tongue, it will probably change still further—that it is, in fact, in a constant state of flux. The literary form of language is that upon which the attention is almost exclusively concentrated, and the student naturally learns to regard language as something fixed and unchanging. He is not encouraged to ask the reason for the rules which he has to master, and must be content with the explanation which comes so readily from the teacher’s tongue: that some apparent exception to the general rule was made—deliberately, for all that he hears to the contrary—‘for the sake of euphony.’ It is but rarely suggested that some puzzling rule of ‘letter’ change in Latin or Greek is based upon the speech habits of the Romans or Greeks hundreds—perhaps thousands—of years before the Classical Period of those languages, or that the conditions under which the ‘exceptional’ form occurs differ, in a way that can be ascertained, from those which produce the ‘normal’ form.

It is not intended, in the above remarks, to criticise adversely the methods employed in teaching the Classics to the very young; the age at which scientific explanations of linguistic facts should be given is a question for educationists DIFFERENT VIEWS OF LANGUAGE to decide. All that it is for the moment desired to emphasize is that the practical study of language differs very considerably from the historical study, in point of view and in method.

Every teacher of the history of English or of any other language knows how difficult it is to convey to young students at the University the first inkling of the historical point of view and method as applied to language.

Nor is this surprising when we consider how different is the way in which one trained in historical methods regards human speech, from that which is the natural standpoint of the practical and literary student of language. To take a few points: the schoolboy has been taught, ‘We ought to pronounce as we spell’; when he begins to study the history of a language he is told, ‘Not at all; we spell in such and such a way, because originally the pronunciation was approximately this or that.’ He has hitherto believed that the written, literary form of language was the real language, and that uttered speech was a rather lame attempt to follow the former; instead of this view receiving confirmation from his new teachers, he is asked to discard it completely, to think of language as something which is primarily uttered and heard, and to banish, for the time being, from his mind the fact that writing has been invented. Again, whereas the young student has probably gathered that ‘rules’ of speech were made by grammarians, and therefore must be obeyed, he now hears that the grammarians have absolutely no authority to prescribe what is ‘right’ or ‘wrong,’ but can merely state what is the actual usage, and that they are good or bad grammarians according as they report truthfully on this point.

To many people ‘exceptions’ to grammatical rules are as the breath of their nostrils, and ‘irregularities’ in language are a source of income. It is therefore disconcerting to a youth, hitherto bred up in an atmosphere of linguistic chaos, to be told that the entire conception of ‘exceptions’ upon which he has been nourished is fundamentally fallacious, that there is no such thing as real ‘irregularity’ in the historical development of speech, that anomalies are only apparent, that nothing occurs in language without a reason, and that this reason must be sought, even though, in many cases, it elude our pursuit. It is to be hoped that there is nothing unjust in this adumbration of the contrast between what we may call the popular or literary, (in this case they are the same thing) and the philological view of language. The examples given as exhibiting the point of view of one who has never approached the problems of the history of a language are all drawn from the personal experience of a teacher.

We may now endeavour to state rather more fully the main considerations upon which the method of historical linguistic study at the present time is based. The general method pursued is the outcome of the views now held concerning the nature of language, and the conditions under which it lives and grows.

By the history of a language is meant an account of its development in all its dialects, of all the changes which these have undergone, from the earliest period at which it is possible to obtain any knowledge of them, down to the latest. This investigation demands the formulation, so THE HISTORICAL VIEW far as possible, of the laws of change which obtain at any given moment in the language—that is, a statement of each tendency to change as it arises, and an examination of the factors and conditions of each tendency. Now, all knowledge of any period of a language other than the present, must necessarily be obtained from written documents. What we are investigating, however, is the life-history of the language itself—that is, of the feelings and ideas of the people, as they have been handed on and modified through the ages, and of one of the most direct and expressive symbols of these, namely, the various sounds formed by the organs of speech. Uttered speech is itself a mere set of symbols of certain states of consciousness; a mode of expression often less direct than a gesture, a picture, or a statue, since these can represent a passion, a wish, or a memory of an event in such a way that they may be of universal significance. The symbol in these cases is self-interpretative. The symbols of speech, however, are only intelligible to those to whom they have become familiar by custom, and who associate the same groups of ideas with the sounds. Uttered speech, therefore, is an indirect and symbolic mode of conveying impressions from one mind to another; but written language is more indirect still, for it is but the symbol of a symbol. Until the written record is interpreted, and converted into the sounds which it symbolizes, it means nothing; it does not become language.

This process of interpretation has to be carried out, and the veil of symbolism rent asunder, before we can arrive, in dealing with the records of the past, at the actual subject of our investigation. We must never lose sight of the true aim of our search—the spoken sound, which is the outward and audible part of language. It is clear that the degree of success with which we reconstruct the earlier stages of a language, and therefore the measure of accuracy in our views of its history, depends to a very large extent upon our power of interpreting correctly the written symbols, and of making them live as sounds.

But, however successful may be our attempts at revivifying the past history of a language, so long as we confine ourselves to a single tongue the limits of possibility are reached comparatively soon—the record fails us often just when we most need it. In tracing back the history of English, we have a series of documents which stretch back for more than twelve hundred years. During this period the language has undergone many changes—in sounds, in vocabulary, in accidence, and in the structure of the sentence. The earlier writings, in so far as they are, within the limits of possibility, a faithful record of what was actually the condition of English at different stages of development, enable us to observe the rise and passing away of various habits of speech and tendencies to change. Thus, for instance, we can understand why ‘breath’ (brɛþ) has a voiceless final consonant, and ‘breathe’ (brīð) a voiced, since we can show that the latter word had an earlier form, O.E. brœ̄þan or brēþan (inf.), whereas the O.E. form of the former was brœ̄þ or brēþ; and, further, that voiceless open consonants were voiced in O.E. medially between vowels, but remained voiceless when final. The voiced sound in ‘breathe’ is therefore due to a change which took place hundreds of years ago, when the verbal forms still PAST INTERPRETS PRESENT retained their suffixes, and when þ was followed by a vowel. In the same way we need not go beyond our own language to understand the difference of vowels between the singular ‘child’ and the plural ‘children.’ In this case, as in the former, there is nothing in the spelling of the two forms to indicate a difference of pronunciation. In O.E. the singular was ċild, which originally had a short vowel. Before the end of the O.E. period, however (by 1050 probably), short vowels were lengthened before the combination -ld. This old long ī developed quite regularly into our present diphthong (ai). This lengthening, however, did not take place when the combination -ld- was followed by a third consonant. The O.E. plural of this word was ċildru, which in M.E. appears as childre side by side with the weak form children, both of which forms retained the old short ĭ sound. This sound has remained unchanged down to the present time. The differences between singular and plural here, therefore, are due to the presence or absence respectively, of the conditions of vowel-lengthening in O.E.

On the other hand,there is a vast number of phenomena whose explanation cannot be found within the history of English itself, because their causes lie further back than the period of the oldest English records. The substantive ‘doom’ (dūm) is related to the verb ‘deem,’ the former being normally developed from O.E. dōm, the latter from O.E. dēman. Here the difference exists already in the oldest form of English of which we have any direct knowledge. We might surmise, perhaps, that the relation of the two vowels (ū) and (ī) in these words was identical with that between those of the words ‘tooth’ (tūþ), plural ‘teeth’ (tīþ), or goose (gūs), geese (gīs), which in O.E. are tōþ, tēþ, gōs, gēs, respectively. Since the differences here are already well established in the earliest form of English which has come down to us, we are unable to decide from a consideration of that language by itself whether this vowel difference is original—whether, that is, from time immemorial there have always been two distinct forms of the roots of these words, or whether the differences arose at a later date. In the latter case we should assume that, owing to causes which cannot be traced in the O.E. period as we know it, one original vowel had been differentiated into two quite separate sounds. Is there any way of getting beyond the written documents of English and settling this question? Can we by any means reconstruct the forms as they existed before they were separated? Assuming that the differences are not primitive, can we supply the missing link which O.E. cannot reveal? The answer is to be found in the wider survey of other cognate languages, known as the Science of Comparative Philology. It has been universally accepted since Franz Bopp founded scientific philology, that what are known as the Aryan or Indo-Germanic languages, are a group of speech-families descended, or developed from a common ancestor. English, as is well known, is a member of the Germanic family of this group. By a minute comparison of the peculiarities of all the sister languages of a family, comparative philology endeavours to gain a knowledge of a form older than any of them—their common ancestor. In the case of English we should first try, by comparing the Germanic tongues, to reconstruct parent Germanic, and then, by a similar process of comparison of this with the ancestral COMPARATIVE AND HISTORICAL GRAMMAR forms of other Aryan families—Indian, Greek, Italic, Slavonic, etc.—to reach some conception of the source of all, the Primitive Aryan mother-tongue. The methods of comparison and reconstruction will be discussed later on, and it is sufficient here to point out the close relationship between historical and comparative grammar. The latter is, indeed, only an extension of the former; it carries the study of the history of a single language further back, and seeks to shed more light upon it by investigating the habits and nature of its sisters, cousins, parents, and grandparents. We may consider Aryan speech as one vast and living stream of language, which has flowed into many different branching channels. These, again, fork out into innumerable rivulets.

Languages which have been separate for thousands of years have altered so much from their original form, and have developed on such different lines, that they are often absolutely unrecognisable as relatives; but, nevertheless, we may reflect that English, as it is spoken to-day, has reached its present form by being passed on from mouth to mouth for thousands of years, from a time when it began to vary from a tongue which had in it the potentialities not only of English, but also of Greek, of Slavonic, and Celtic. Every family of languages, each individual of the family, has its peculiar habits and tendencies of development. One language may very early lose a feature which another will preserve for ages. Again, a certain characteristic may disappear from a language, leaving behind it, however, a trace of its existence. In this case we can see the result, but not the cause, nor can we account for the result until we find that some other language has preserved the feature in question. The change of vowels in O.E. dōm, dēman, etc., can easily be accounted for by a comparison with the other Germanic languages, which show that the O.E, noun preserves the original vowel ō, which has been changed in O.E. from a back to a front vowel through the influence of a front consonant (j) which has disappeared in that language, although it is preserved in Gothic dōmjan, Old High German tuomian. This particular kind of change, known as i-mutation, occurs in hundreds of words in O.E., though, as a rule, the i or j which caused the fronting, disappeared before the English period, leaving only the effects of its original presence, which can be demonstrated, however, from cognate languages.

In the historical study of a language we are perpetually brought face to face with problems, the solution of which requires not only a careful sifting of evidence, but a trained judgment in drawing conclusions therefrom. To deal successfully with historical linguistic problems the critical faculty needs to be formed and strengthened by contact with the actualities of living speech, and clarified by a knowledge of the general conditions which govern the development of all language.

Of late years some understanding of the general principles of speech development has come to be regarded as essential to the fruitful study or just conception of the history of any language. It is now commonly held that the best way to form sound general views as to the nature of speech-life is to study the facts of living language, especially as they are displayed most familiarly in the speech habits of ourselves and our contemporaries. These STARTING-POINT OF THE STUDY facts, which we can observe directly, are the best key to the understanding of those forces which helped to mould language in the past, since there is no reason for believing that the conditions under which human speech existed and developed in bygone ages were essentially different from those which obtain at the present day. We should endeavour, therefore, to realize what the ‘life’ of language really is by the practical study and observation of a living tongue, and, further, that tendencies to modify language, such as we may discover in ourselves, have always been in operation; in other words, the process of the evolution of language is always going on, and the factors which direct it are of the same kind in all periods.

The life of language has two aspects—the facts of human consciousness, which are the subject of psychological investigation, and the facts connected with the mode of expression, which in the case of speech are the sounds which result from the movements of the vocal organs. This latter group of facts are the subject of a special branch of physiological inquiry, that of practical Phonetics.

If linguistic study be confined to a purely literary form of language, and especially to the literary forms of the ancient languages, there is a tendency for the student to get into the habit of considering language as something cut and dried, and fixed once for all in a definite mould.

We are apt to forget that all literary languages are, to a certain extent, artificial products. They are deliberate, and bound by tradition, and they lack the spontaneity of unstudied, natural utterance. The development of literary dialects will be discussed hereafter, but it may be pointed out here that this form of language is slowly evolved from the spoken language, and is in all cases behind this in development, in the sense of being more archaic, and generally less flexible and adaptable. Any new departure in the literary language can only come from the spoken form. In the case of languages which are no longer spoken, and which therefore depend entirely upon literary tradition, development is impossible. In the case of Latin, for instance, which is still largely cultivated as a literary vehicle, it is obvious that no innovation can take place, except, indeed, by the incorporation into Latin style of the idiom of the writer’s native tongue, which was largely done by mediæval writers, and possibly, quite unconsciously, at the present day also, even by good scholars. Such innovations as this, however, do not change real classical Latin itself, and are rightly regarded as ‘corruptions.’ There is no possible source of Latin except genuine Latin authors; all potentialities of normal development are at an end, and Latin prose, when written at the present day, can only be a reproduction of well-authenticated modes of expression, for which sanction can be found in the classical writers.

The literary form of a language which is still spoken, however, is forever receiving fresh life from the colloquial speech. As new words or expressions come into use in the spoken language, they are gradually promoted to a place in the language of literature, and they often remain in use here after they have ceased to be employed in the ordinary colloquial speech of everyday life. Thus the written form of a living language does not become fixed, but is forever undergoing regeneration and rejuvenation. But this new THE LITERARY BIAS life comes primarily from the spoken language.

Another unfortunate view which the exclusive study of the literary language gives rise to, is that which regards speech as something with a life of its own, something which can exist apart from those who speak it. That which is written remains: scratched on parchment or graven upon stone, the symbols of written language may endure for countless ages. This permanence and independence of the symbol has led men to attribute the same character to that for which it stands.

Now, it is an essential element in the conception which scholars at the present day have of language, that it does not exist by itself, and apart from the speakers. This conception brings us back to the importance of spoken language, for this can only be reached through the speakers themselves. The study of speech, as has been indicated, involves, first, that of certain psychological processes, and, secondly, that of the symbol and expression of these—that is, of speech sounds, which are the result of certain series of bodily activities.

The outward and audible part of language, the symbol of what is inward and of the mind, can be reached directly and immediately; it can be observed in others as well as in ourselves. The psychological side of language can only be studied directly and immediately by the analysis of our own consciousness. From the use of intelligible symbols we are able to infer in other minds the same mental processes and conceptions as those which exist in our own. For these reasons we insist upon the importance of the careful study of spoken language generally, and also in particular, upon that of our own speech in both aspects.

Spoken language is the natural expression of the personality of living human beings; from the nature of the case, this must vary along with the change of their mental and bodily habits. A nation, a small community, or an individual, is continually gaining new experiences, feeling new aspirations, discovering fresh needs. All these conditions find expression in their speech. Speakers form fresh associations, and gradually come to use old words in a new way. The history of a single language yields innumerable instances of change in the meanings of words. Or words fall out of use, because for some reason they are superfluous. Again, contact with other nations is the means of introducing foreign words into the native vocabulary, both for things and ideas which are quite primitive and familiar, and for those which pass into the national consciousness as knowledge and experience widen. In the domain of vocabulary there is a perpetual losing, gaining, and readaptation of material.

Nor does pronunciation stand still in a living language. Speech sounds are the result of certain bodily movements, which we may consider as a group of physical habits. The habitual movements of the vocal organs vary from generation to generation, and so, therefore, do the sounds which result from them. Up to a certain point of literary development, the written form of a language records, approximately, the changes of pronunciation, though the record is probably always some way behind the actual facts, after the first attempts to write the language down have been made. But after a time a fixed method of spelling is introduced, with which the pronunciation grows SOUND CHANGE more and more out of harmony as time goes on. In English, the main features of our spelling became fixed in the sixteenth century, so that the far-reaching changes in our pronunciation which took place during the next three centuries are, of course, unrecorded in our orthography.

The principles and possibilities of sound change, which are so vitally important in modern philology, can only be really grasped by those who have investigated, in their own speech, the processes of articulation, and have observed how these tend to vary.

Before leaving, for the moment, the question of change in pronunciation in living speech, we may consider a little more fully the importance of a phonetic training for the student of the history of his own or any other tongue. We have just seen that sound change is a process which is always going on in language, and it has been noted that the interpretation of the written symbols of the past plays a very large part in historical linguistic study; and, further, in judging of what took place in the past, we need the help of our actual experience of the present. This is especially true of theories of the change of sounds, for unless these changes can be realized in a practical way, our account of the development of speech forms degenerates into a mere algebraic equation, far removed from the real, living facts. Now, if these assertions are true it follows that a general knowledge of the processes upon which speech sounds depend, and some power to discriminate varieties of sound is essential to the scientific study of language. One result of the one-sided view of language which is almost universal in this country is that hardly anybody really knows what his own speech is like. Most people think of language in terms of black symbols on white paper, and not in terms of sounds at all.

They even go the length of pretending that they can hear a difference between such pairs as horsehoarse, ParmaPalmer, kernelcolonel, and so on. Of course, a difference can easily be made; pronunciation can be ‘faked’ to any extent. The point is that in ordinary educated English speech in the South, there is no difference between the above pairs.

Phonetics is still regarded by the majority of educated persons as either a fad, or a fraud, possibly a pious one. If it is insisted that more attention should be paid, in the teaching of English, to the ‘spoken language,’ there is an outcry to the effect that English literature is one of the noblest of human achievements, that the ordinary speech of children and even of grown-up people is full of vulgarisms, mistakes in grammar, and solecisms of every sort, and that by dwelling upon English as it is spoken, these errors will merely be confirmed. English, it is urged, is seen at its noblest in the works of the great writers; these should form the sole subject of English studies. To suggest a scientific way of investigating the sounds of the language which we speak, rouses antipathy and opposition.

It is, of course, easy to find reasons against that which we cannot or will not understand. Thus when, a few years ago, the Scotch Education Department introduced phonetics into the list of subjects to be studied in the training colleges, arguments of the most conflicting nature were urged against the measure. The present writer has the best reason for knowing that, whereas one party POPULAR MISCONCEPTIONS held that it was preposterous for the Department to try and ‘improve’ Scottish speech by insisting upon the adoption of English models of pronunciation, others objected chiefly because, they said, to dwell upon what actually occurred in Scotch pronunciation, instead of insisting upon what ought to occur, would tend to confirm and perpetuate the vulgarisms.

As both of these objections, or similar ones, are probably urged not only in Scotland, but also in this country, against the study of phonetics, it is, perhaps, worth while to answer them. In the first place, it should be said that by the study of phonetics is not meant the attempt to introduce this or that pronunciation, but simply a study of the actual movements of the vocal organs which result in the various sounds of human speech. A phonetic training involves, then, no more than development of the power of discriminating between different sounds, and a knowledge of how the sounds are made. If we could hear all sounds quite accurately, and knew how to reproduce them, we should have no trouble in acquiring the pronunciation of foreign languages. This is perhaps an impossible degree of perfection for most, but a phonetic training will undoubtedly help in the right direction. It may be added that every teacher of languages must needs be to a certain extent a phonetician; he endeavours to teach his pupils to pronounce certain sounds; he pronounces the sound himself, and often tries to explain how this is done. All that is here urged is that he should give right instructions, and not, as is too often the case, a perfectly fantastic account of the position of the tongue, jaws, etc. It should be understood that phonetic study does not involve a preference for this or that manner of pronunciation of English. In fact, the first lesson which the serious student of phonetics has to learn is to take facts as they are, to start with, to begin with his own natural pronunciation, and to attempt to become conscious of the movements of his tongue and lips in framing those sounds which he habitually employs in speaking his native language, without discussing the question of whether his pronunciation is ‘good’ or ‘bad.’ A street arab who had thoroughly mastered the principles of his own ‘speech basis’—that is, of that group of movements and positions of tongue, lips, jaws, etc., which occurred naturally in his manner of speech—and who could accurately describe these, would be a far more competent phonetician than the speaker of a very ‘pure’ and refined form of English who was ignorant of what his own sounds actually were, or of how he made them. This brings us to a consideration of the fallacy that the minute study of one’s own pronunciation, if it happens to be faulty or ‘vulgar,’ will tend to confirm and make more inveterate those defects which it should be our constant endeavour to get rid of. This view is a very common one, and it amounts to saying that if we have a failing or a vice, which we wish to correct, it is better to ignore it, or at most only to have a very vague idea of its precise nature. Whether this principle holds good or not in conduct, or in intellectual habits, we need not discuss here, but it is absolutely certain that it is false in matters of pronunciation. One reason why so many teachers of foreign languages fail to impart an accurate pronunciation to their pupils is that they themselves are so frequently quite unacquainted with the speech VULGARISMS basis of those whom they are teaching. They are unable to say authoritatively, ‘Your English sound is so-and-so, and it is made in such and such a way; this foreign sound for which you are substituting your own sound which strikes your ear as something like it, is so-and-so and it is made in such and such a way, entirely different from that set of articulations which produces the English sound.’ If we wish to master a foreign sound, instead of being content with substituting a sound of our own language which, to the untrained ear, somewhat resembles it, we must thoroughly understand both sounds, so as to discriminate between and contrast, both the sounds themselves, and the vocal movements and positions which produce them.

If, then, it be desired to ‘correct’ the pronunciation of the native language, the same principle holds, for from the moment that the problem is to acquire a new sound, it matters not whether that sound occurs in another form of English or in some remote foreign tongue, the difficulty is of the same kind—namely, to master a new series of movements, or a new combination of movements, of the organs of speech.

Whatever be the case then, in other spheres of thought and conduct, in pronunciation, at any rate, an accurate knowledge of our ‘faults’ is the beginning of ‘improvement’: it is, indeed, a necessary first step.

With regard to the expressions so commonly applied to speech, such as ‘mistake,’ ‘vulgarism,’ ‘corruption,’ and the like, it is inevitable that our views of the propriety of such terms should change in proportion as we learn something concerning the path of development which any language has travelled during a few centuries. The reason for this statement will appear more fully in the course of this book; but it may be said here that most of the abusive terms popularly applied to certain forms of speech have, from the scientific point of view, either no meaning at all, or one which differs widely from that which such terms usually bear.

One who is accustomed to observe how a language changes in the course of centuries; how speakers in one age, or in one province, naturally acquire habits of speech which differ widely from those which obtain at other times and in other geographical areas; how a community tends to modify its speech now in one direction, now in another, sometimes owing to social or other conditions which can be traced, sometimes without any discoverable external cause, one who is an unprejudiced student of the development of human culture as it is expressed in spoken language, is unwilling to assert that one line of development is ‘good,’ while another is ‘bad,’ or to dogmatize as to what ought to be the form which language shall take. If we regard the unfolding of that body of habits which we call ‘language’ as a natural process, one which is for the most part unconscious and independent of the deliberate intention of the speakers, we are content to chronicle what actually exists, and investigate so far as possible how it arose: we do not attempt to adjudge praise or blame to this or that phenomenon. In a word, as students of the history of language, we are concerned purely with the facts, all the facts that we can ascertain, and from them we endeavour to form a clear conception of what is, and of how it arose out of what was.

Do we then, admit no ‘right’ or ‘wrong’ in language CORRECTNESS IN LANGUAGE from this point of view? Certainly we do; only we should define these terms, as Osthoff pointed out years ago (Schriftsprache und Volksmundart, Berlin, 1888, p. 25, etc.), in rather a different way from that popularly accepted. Whatever exists in the natural speech of a community at a given period is right for the speech of that community at that particular moment; it is, whether we like it or not, a fact of the speech history of the community. Any manner of speech—whether pronunciation, word, grammatical inflection, or form of sentence—which is foreign to the natural speech habit of a community at a given period is wrong, so far as the dialect of the moment in that particular community is concerned.

The failure to grasp this simple principle is responsible for the popular misconception of the terms ‘correct’ and ‘incorrect’ speech, and the consequent misuse of them.

What usually happens is that the critic of language has in his mind a vague picture of an ideal standard of language, probably based on his own vague notion of the way he speaks himself, and he proceeds to test all other modes of speech by this standard. If other speakers appear to the censor to approximate to his own standard, he approves them as ‘good’ or ‘correct’ speakers; if he gathers that they deviate from the model which he has set up, then they are set down as being ‘corrupt,’ ‘incorrect,’ or even ‘vulgar.’ But he does not realize that those who speak differently from himself are not pretending, for the most part, that they are speaking in the same way as he does. They are quite frankly using the natural dialect of another geographical area, another suburb, it may be, or of a different social class. Probably each man who comes under the condemnation of our critic is, as a matter of fact, speaking his own dialect quite ‘correctly’ from the point of view mentioned above. On the other hand, a mixture of dialects is not infrequently heard. A speaker tries to adopt the speech of what he considers a more refined or more elevated sphere than that which is customary to him, and occasionally reverts to his own natural way of speaking—to his native dialect, in fact. The error in judging of such cases lies in not realizing that every form of speech, whether it be a provincial or a class dialect, has a perfectly good reason for existing and for being as it is; each has its own history, and has followed its own path of development. According to this view, therefore, each dialect is equally ‘good’ and equally ‘correct.’ There are, however, two tests by which the relative superiority of different dialects may be gauged—the one real and absolute, the other artificial and a matter of convention.

A language may justifiably be judged, and its merits appreciated, according to its qualities as a medium of expression. The degree of expressiveness which a language possesses is its true claim to respect. If it can be shown that one form of speech is more flexible, more adaptable to the needs of those who speak it, more capable of expressing subtle shades of thought and feeling than another, then we may surely say that it is the finer language of the two.

The other test of superiority, which we have called artificial and conventional, has a very real existence in English—namely, the test of what is received and recognised as the ‘correct’ form of speech in polite and STANDARD ENGLISH cultivated society. From the purely scientific point of view, as has been already set forth, no difference of superiority can be recognised between the speech heard at the bench of a village ale-house and that of the Bench of Bishops. But according to the actual feelings of English society, that of the latter is the more distinguished, graceful, and desirable. It is a fact which nothing can alter, that there is a form of English which enjoys a prestige, and a place in the general estimation of which nothing can rob it. This form of English is essentially a class dialect; it is independent, or largely independent, of locality; it is the form of English which obtains, with an astonishing degree of uniformity, among the upper and upper middle classes of this country, and it may be heard with the same purity in Durham, York, Newcastle, or Birmingham, as in London, Oxford, or Cambridge. So greatly is this standard English prized, that those who have not acquired it from the cradle upwards, usually take pains to do so in later life, and there can be no doubt that it is convenient for those who wish to enter the public services or to take part in distinguished social gatherings to possess it, or at least a good imitation of it. Those who have spoken from childhood this colourless form of English, free from provincial peculiarities, devoid of the rasping sound of inverted r before consonants, with no tendency to shaky initial aspirates in stressed words, or even in words which have only a secondary stress, no undue mouthing or over-emphatic utterance, not unnaturally regard it as the purest, most harmonious, and most refined form of English speech. This view of a language, however, is purely a matter of custom; we always admire most what we are accustomed to hear and to use ourselves. Such an estimate has no absolute value, but is entirely relative and subjective. Speakers of Northern English and Scotch speakers often consider standard English as mincing and affected, in some cases even (e.g., the loss of the r-sound before consonants) as slipshod and almost vulgar. So much for habit.

The historical position of this polite form of English is that it is a very mixed dialect, which, by a variety of social and political circumstances, has acquired prominence over all other English dialects by becoming the language of Literature (for the written language is largely based upon it), of the Court, of the aristocracy, of the Law, the Church, the Legislature, and the Stage. It is probable that the Metropolis, Oxford, and the East Midlands all contributed to its origin, while the remoter influences of the North and the extreme South have both helped to shape it. We shall have to consider the rise of this dialect more in detail later on. It might probably be maintained with considerable plausibility that, owing to the circumstances of its history, the standard dialect, which of all forms of spoken English approximates most nearly to the written language, has an absolute superiority to any other dialect of our language as a means of expression, excepting always some of the dialects of Scotland. At the same time, it may perhaps temper the enthusiasm of some to remind them that standard English is not nearly so uniform in its sounds or in its other characteristics as a superficial observer might imagine, and, further, that the standard varies considerably from generation to generation; for instance, much that was very ‘good form’ as recently as THE NATURAL STARTING-POST the end of the eighteenth century would now be considered ‘vulgar’ or ‘provincial’ even by speakers who are not over-fastidious. The pronunciations ‘sarvant,’ ‘goold’ (gūld), ‘chaney tay-pot’ (tʃēni tēpot), and the frequent use of the pronoun ’em (əm), may serve as examples of this fact in the meantime.

The upshot of the foregoing remarks is that we may keep our natural preferences for this or that English dialect, but we must not ignore the fact that other dialects exist, and we should admit that it is not wise to abuse them, simply because they differ from the form that we ourselves use.

It is very important for the student to recognise and observe differences in English speech, and to contrast and compare them. The problem of English philology lies within the differences and agreements of the various English dialects, and questions at issue are the origin, history, and mutual relations of these.

Within the limits of such an investigation, questions arise which contain the germ of all comparative philology; the methods pursued in dealing with the history of the English dialects are those which it is also desirable to pursue in considering the relations of the great Aryan families of languages.

The study of the native tongue, beginning with its spoken forms, and proceeding thence to inquire into the why and wherefore of what exists, is therefore the best introduction to the advanced study of Aryan philology in its widest sense. All the principles of linguistic development, all the factors of evolution, exist ready for our observation in the living speech of our own English dialects; and while, as has been said, the discipline afforded by their study is a preparation for the larger science, it should be borne in mind that this study cannot be profitably pursued unless the same accuracy of method, and the same exactness of observation be applied in both cases, and, above all, unless the same scientific spirit and the same general conception of the life of language animate all our inquiries.


CHAPTER II

THE SOUNDS OF SPEECH[A]

Phonetics, or the science of speech sounds, involves a two-fold training—that of the ear to discriminate minute shades of difference in sound, and that of the vocal organs to reproduce these. The former is only gained by the repeated hearing of varieties of sound and a keen and patient observation; the latter by a knowledge of the processes of articulation and a careful cultivation of the power of recognising the muscular sensations associated with the different movements and positions of the vocal organs in speech.

This power of recognition, which is almost lacking in untrained persons, must be based, primarily, upon the observation of one’s own speech. To gain the power to analyze and describe the movements of the vocal organs in uttering the most familiar sounds of our own language is to make the first steps in a real knowledge of scientific and practical phonetics.


The letters placed in brackets in the following pages are the Phonetic Symbols of the sounds referred to.

Anything like a complete treatise on phonetics would be out of place in such a work as this, and no more is here attempted than to give a brief outline of the classification of speech sounds according to the Organic Method, as set forth in the system of Melville Bell, the author of Visible Speech, and made more scientific and exact by Mr. Sweet. For a full treatment of the subject the student may refer to Sweet’s Primer of Phonetics (third edition), History of English Sounds, 1888, and to Sievers’ Phonetik (fourth edition). The student will be well advised to approach the study of phonetics with the help of a teacher, and also to master one system thoroughly before coquetting with others, as the result of reading a series of treatises by different writers is usually to produce confusion of mind, no proper grasp of any system, and no gain in the control of the speech organs.

The classification of speech sounds according to the organic system is based upon a consideration of the position and condition of those organs which produce the sounds. It is an axiom that the same sound can only be uttered in one way—that is, by a given mode of activity of a particular organ. If the position and the mode of activity be altered ever so little, a different sound is the result. The limit of discrimination of minute differences of position and sound is that of delicacy of ear and muscular sensation.

The organs which play a part in the production of the sounds of speech are: The Lungs, from which the air-stream passes through the glottis, mouth, and nose; the Diaphragm, the muscle which controls the volume and force of the air-stream; the Glottis; the Mouth cavity; the Hard and Soft Palates; the Nose; the Tongue; and the Lips. The Jaws are important, especially the movable lower jaw, since the tongue is raised or lowered in conjunction with it; and the teeth and gums, since they PRINCIPLES OF CLASSIFICATION contribute to the formation of sounds, with the aid of the lips and tongue.

We may consider briefly the activities of those organs of speech which can be moved at will.

The Glottis contains the Vocal Chords, which can be either stretched across it so as to close it, or folded back so as to leave it completely open.

In the former case, if the air be driven through, the vocal chords vibrate, as the air-stream forces its way between them.

The sound caused by the air passing through the closed glottis, and setting up vibration in the vocal chords, is technically known as Voice. This vibration accompanies most vowels in ordinary ‘loud’ speech, and a great number of consonants, such as z, v, and th in ‘this’ (ð).

When the air-stream passes through the open glottis, and the chords do not vibrate, as in the ordinary sigh, the sound is known as Breath, as in s, f, th in ‘think’ (þ).

A third possibility is Whisper, in which the glottis is definitely contracted and narrowed, but the vocal chords are not tightened, and do not vibrate.

The Soft Palate or Velum, from which the uvula depends, serves to open or close the nose passage, and probably also acts in sympathetic relation to certain movements of the tongue.

The Uvula in certain sounds, such as the usual French r, trills against the back of the tongue, which in this case is raised.

The Nose Passage is open in the so-called nasal sounds, such as the consonants n, m, ng (ŋ) in ‘sing’ (siŋ), or in the nasalized vowels so frequent in French, as in ‘bon’ (bɔ̃), ‘fin’ (fæ̃), ‘un’ (œ̃), etc. In these cases the air-stream passes through the nose passage. In the nasal vowels the stream passes through mouth and nose at once, in n, m, only through the latter.

In other than nasal sounds the nose passage is closed by the soft palate.

The Tongue is, perhaps, the most important, as it certainly is the most active, of the vocal organs.

The tongue can move chiefly in four ways: inwards and outwards—that is, it can be retracted or advanced; up and down—that is, it can be raised or lowered.

If the tongue be retracted or drawn back, the back part, or even the root, is brought into play; if it be advanced or thrust forwards towards the front teeth, the forward part or the tip comes into activity.

In considering the raising or lowering of the tongue, we distinguish different degrees of Height, which, as we shall see, are of great significance in determining the sound of vowels.

In addition to the direction of the movements of the tongue, we have also to take account of the particular part or area involved in uttering a given sound.

Beginning from the back of the mouth, we distinguish the Root; the Back; the Front or Middle of the tongue; the Blade, which is that portion which lies between the middle and the Point or tip; and, lastly, the Point itself.

Each of these areas functions in the production of speech sounds, and their several activities are associated with characteristic sounds.

The Lips are the most easily observed of all the ACTIVITIES OF THE ORGANS OF SPEECH movable organs of speech. They may be drawn back from the teeth so as almost to expose these, as in French i in ‘fini,’ or they may be protruded or pouted. The lips can function in the formation both of vowels and consonants; in the former case they always act in conjunction with the tongue, in the latter they may act either in conjunction with the tongue, independently of any other organ, or by a combination of the lower lip and the upper teeth.

Distinction between Vowels and Consonants.

By a Consonant we understand a speech sound in which the air-stream is either completely stopped for a moment, as (b, d, g) (in ‘good,’ etc.), or in the formation of which the passage is so far narrowed that there is a distinct friction set up as the air-stream passes out.

In a true Vowel the air-passage is never sufficiently narrowed to produce such friction, although in the case of certain vowels, such as (i) or (u), the narrowing of the air-passage is so great that, under certain conditions, as when the air-stream is forced through with great vigour, an appreciable friction results. In this case the sound ceases to be a pure vowel sound, and becomes consonantal. In pronouncing such words as ‘sea’ many speakers make the final vowel into a weak Open consonant, with a distinct ‘buzz,’ uttering (sij) instead of (sī).

It is best to begin the study of speech sounds with the consonants, as the positions of the vocal organs in pronouncing these sounds are more easily realized by the student.

The Classification of Consonants.

In considering any given consonant, we have to determine the following points: (A) The organ or organs with which the sound is formed, and, if the tongue be used, also the particular area which functions; (B) the mode of activity; (C) whether the articulation is or is not accompanied by Voice—that is, by vibration of the Vocal Chords.

A. The Organs and Area.—From this point of view we have first of all to determine whether the particular consonant we are considering is formed in the Throat (by a contraction below the Glottis); by one of the areas of the Tongue already described—Back, Front, Blade, etc.; by the Lips; or by a combination of more than one organ, such as the Tongue and Lips.

B. The Mode of Activity.—From this point of view we distinguish the following classes:

(1) Open Consonants, in which the mouth passage is sufficiently narrowed to produce a very distinct friction, the air-stream, however, continuing to pass so long as the position is maintained and the air driven from the lungs. This friction may be made at any part of the passage along its whole length—below the glottis in the case of throat consonants, above the glottis by every part of the tongue, by the lips, or by approximating one of the lips to the teeth. Examples of open consonants are—‘ch’ in Scotch ‘loch’ (χ), made between the Back of the Tongue and the Soft Palate (Back-Open); s (ς) made between the Blade of the Tongue and the Hard Palate (Blade-Open); th (þ) in ‘think,’ made between the Point of the tongue and the Teeth (Point-Teeth-Open); and so on.

(2) Stops, or Stop Consonants, in which the passage is MODE OF FORMATION for a moment completely closed, and then suddenly opened, so that the air bursts forth with a certain puff. These are popularly called Explosives. This stopping of the passage may, like the narrowing in (1), he made anywhere along the whole length of the passage. A few examples of stops are (k), made by Back of Tongue and Hard Palate (Back-stop); English (t), made between Point of Tongue and Gums just behind upper teeth (Point-Stop); (p) made by the lips (Lip-Stop).

(3) Nasal Consonants, which are formed, as has been already said, by allowing the air-stream to pass through the nose passage. In the case of the English nasal consonants the mouth-passage is always closed, so that (n) is really a nasalized (d)—that is, Point-(Stop)-Nasal; but any open consonant may also be nasalized, in which case the air passes through both nose and mouth at the same time. Besides n, English has m, formed by the lips (Lip-Nasal), and ng, as in ‘sing’ (ŋ, Back-Nasal), formed by the back of the tongue against the soft palate. Thus (m) is merely a nasalized (b), and (ŋ) a nasalized (g).

(4) Divided or Side Consonants.—This class is chiefly typified by the l-sounds, which are made by the tongue forming a partial stoppage, in such a way as to permit the air-stream to escape on either side of the point of contact. English (l) is usually formed by the tongue in contact with the gums just behind the upper teeth, in exactly the same way as ordinary English (d), except that, whereas in this case the closure is complete, in that of (l) the edges of the tongue on either side of the point of contact are so far removed from the gums as to allow the air-stream to pass all the time in the manner just described. Some speakers, notably the Welsh, form contact with only one side of the tongue, so that the air passes out between the other side of the tongue and the gums or teeth. Hence the name Side consonant. This kind of Divided articulation can be carried out between any area of the tongue and the palate. Thus we have in some languages, e.g., Russian, a back-divided consonant—that is, an l formed with the same part of the tongue as that which forms the back-stop (g).

(5) Trills.—This name explains itself, and the typical trilled sounds are the r-series. In Scotch r it is the point of the tongue which trills just behind the teeth; in French r it is the Uvula which trills upon the back of the tongue. In Southern English there is normally no trill, no ‘rolling’ of the r, the sound being usually some variety of weak point-open consonant.

C. Voice and Breath.—These terms, which refer respectively to the activity and passivity of the vocal chords, have already been explained. The vibration of the vocal chords, which we call Voice, produces a very characteristic sound, sometimes called ‘buzz,’ and the vibration can easily be felt if the fingers are placed upon the ‘Adam’s Apple’ while such sounds as (z, v, or ð) are uttered with a certain loudness. Open consonants are the best for this purpose, because they can be prolonged to any extent—so long, indeed, as the supply of air from the lungs holds out.

Each and every consonant position may be either accompanied by vibration of the vocal chords or the reverse; that is to say, that every consonant may be either Voiced or un-Voiced. It does not follow that any given language possesses both voiced and voiceless varieties of all its consonants. Thus in English we have no entirely voiceless l, although this is common in Welsh, where it is CONSONANTS IN NATURAL SERIES expressed by ll, as in Llandudno, etc.; while in German the voiced form of ‘sh,’ as in ship ʃ, does not exist, and causes Germans great trouble, although it is frequent in French, where it is written ‘j,’ as in ‘jamais’ (žamɛ), etc., and occurs also in English in such words as ‘pleasure’ (plϵžə).

One of the first exercises which the beginner should practise is that of unvoicing voiced, and voicing unvoiced consonants. This implies such control of the glottis that it am be consciously and deliberately opened and closed at will. When the student has thoroughly mastered this process, he will find that he has added considerably to his range of easily articulated sounds.

In describing a consonantal sound it is usually only necessary to mention the fact when it is Voiced, it being assumed that such is not the case if nothing is said about it. Thus (g) is described as the back-stop-voice, while the corresponding Breath or Voiceless sound is described simply as back-stop.

In studying the consonants it is convenient to take them in their natural series; thus, if we begin with the back consonants, we have the following table:

Back (Voiced).Back (Voiceless).
Openʓ, as in Gm. sorgeχ, as in Scot. loch
Stopg, as in goodk, as in car, or king
Nasalŋ, as in singŋ̥, —
Dividedɫ, as in Russ. (ɫoʃad), ‘horse’ɫ̥, —
Trillr, as in Fr. rendrer̥, as in Fr. français

The advantage of this method of practice is, that not only is it exhaustive, since it considers all the possible consonants—at least, in type—of the group, but it also impresses upon the student the natural relationship of consonants which are formed in the same part of the mouth, although in different ways; and, further, if the sounds are practised in order, it helps to make him conscious of the processes of articulation.

The beginner starts with the familiar sounds of the series, and gradually learns the unfamiliar ones by acquiring the power to use his organs of speech in new ways. In the back-voice series only two of the series are familiar to most English speakers—(g) and (ŋ)—but, taking these as a starting—point, the student, by closely observing his muscular sensations, so learns to form the Open and the Divided with the same part of the tongue which he uses in forming the Stop and the Nasal. The power of unvoicing depends upon the degree of control which the beginner has over his vocal chords. The back-trill will probably require considerable practice before it can be formed easily and perfectly, and without making faces. The student will find, as a rule, that the utterance of a new sound, the position for which he has only imperfectly mastered, has at first a peculiar ghastliness and hollowness in the effect which it makes upon the ear. This is due to the fact that the organs of speech are in what is to them an unnatural position, which they cannot maintain with ease—in fact, the performance is at first a clumsy one.

It is important that teachers, at any rate, should acquire by practice the power of forming all the sounds with which they deal, clearly, easily, and with precision, as this CLASSIFICATION OF VOWELS gives confidence to the learner.

Full tables of the consonants, and minute accounts of each variety, are given in the works by Sweet and Sievers mentioned above.

The Vowels.

There are four main points to be considered in the analysis of vowel sounds. The peculiar acoustic character of a vowel sound depends upon: A. The height of the tongue; B. the part of the tongue which functions; C. the degree of tenseness of the tongue; D. the position of the lips. If we know these four points with regard to any particular vowel, and can put them into effect with our own vocal organs, then we can both pronounce the vowel ourselves, and so describe it that there can be no doubt as to the precise sound we mean.

We will briefly consider the points in the above order.

A. The Height of the Tongue.—We have already said that the tongue can be either raised or lowered. We distinguish three main degrees of HeightHigh, Mid, Low. Each of these positions may be taken by the back, the front, or the whole of the tongue. Thus we have a high-back, a mid-back, and a low-back vowel, and similarly with the front and mixed or flat vowels.

B. The Part of the Tongue which Functions.—It has been already said that if the tongue be retracted the back part comes into play, and that if it be advanced the front is brought into activity. If the tongue be neither retracted nor advanced, but remain approximately flat in the mouth, then neither back nor front predominates, but the tongue is used along its whole length. From this point of view, therefore, we distinguish the possibilities: vowels made by the Back of the tongue—Back-vowels; those made with the Front of the tongue—Front-vowels; and vowels formed by the Whole of the tongue—Flat or Mixed vowels. A typical back vowel in English is the (ā) in ‘father’ (fāðə), a front is the (ī) in ‘see’ (sī), and a mixed or flat vowel is the vowel in bird (bʌ̄d). To realize the backward and forward movement of the tongue, the student may pronounce in a whisper, or articulate silently, the sound (ū) (as in ‘boot’), and (ī) (as in ‘see’), or, better, the French u (y) in ‘lune’ alternately, (u-y, u-y, u-y), several times, when he will at once become conscious of the sawing backwards and forwards movements.

The front-slack series is the best for the beginner to practise, to realize the height of the tongue; because most Southern English speakers have all three vowels in their normal pronunciation of English.

The following series should be pronounced in order, cave being taken to observe the gradual lowering of the front of the tongue, and the gradual sinking of the lower jaw.

Front.
High(i) in bit
Mid(ɛ) in bet
Low(æ) in bat

The low-front vowel is a great difficulty to Scotch and North of England speakers, who, as a rule, do not possess it in the sounds of their natural speech, but must acquire TENSE AND SLACK CONDITIONS OF THE TONGUE it with great trouble and patience. Such speakers substitute a back vowel, a variety, only short, of the first vowel in ‘father.’ This particular difficulty is one which the uninformed ‘imitation’ method hardly ever overcomes, and many people are irretrievably branded as ‘provincial’ speakers in consequence of their failure to acquire the standard English sound. This is not the expression of a supercilious sense of superiority (there is no particular ethical merit about the low-front vowel), but merely a statement of a scientific fact concerning the dialects of Modern English.

C. The Degree of Tenseness of the Tongue.—For practical purposes it is sufficient to distinguish a tense and a slack condition of the tongue. The muscular sensation which characterizes each may be experienced by pronouncing alternately, and contrasting the accompanying sensations, ee (ī) in ‘see’ and i (i) in ‘sit,’ or French é (e) in ‘été’ with English e (ɛ) in ‘bet.’

The tongue may be either tense or slack while occupying any or all of the before-mentioned positions, so that we have a high-front-tense, a high-front-slack; high-hack-tense, high-back-slack, and so on throughout all the vowels of every series, back, front, and flat.

It should be noted that Mr. Sweet generally uses the terms narrow = tense, and wide = slack, and these terms are probably quite as much used by phoneticians as tense and slack; unfortunately, however, some writers, but imperfectly acquainted with the principles and terminology of the Organic System, have been so far misled by ‘narrow’ and ‘wide’ as to understand them to refer to the narrowing or widening of the mouth passage by raising or lowering the tongue. In other words, they have confused ‘narrowness,’ which merely means tenseness when applied to vowels, with Height, and have gathered that the vowel (i) in ‘bit,’ which Mr. Sweet would call the high-front-wide, is intermediate in position between (ī) in ‘see’ and (e) in ‘été,’ than which nothing is more false.

The important thing for the beginner is thoroughly to understand the terminology which he uses, and to be able to realize by his muscular sensations the processes of which it is descriptive. On the whole, perhaps, tense and slack are to be preferred to narrow and wide, as being more definitely descriptive of the facts.

D. The Position of the Lips.—The action of the lips is obviously quite independent of that of the tongue, so that, no matter how the latter is being employed, the lips may be either passive, whether slightly parted or drawn hack so as to leave the air-stream an unhindered exit, or they may be more or less brought forward or pouted so as to muffle, to a greater or less extent, the air-stream after it passes the teeth.

This pouting or bringing together of the lips is technically known as Rounding, and a vowel thus formed is called a Round or Rounded vowel.

When the student has mastered the processes of retracting and advancing, raising and lowering the tongue at pleasure, he should pass with equal assiduity to that of rounding and unrounding; that is, he should pronounce a vowel sound—for instance, (i) (high-front-tense)—endeavour to feel the position of the tongue, and then, while being careful to maintain this unaltered, he should prolong the vowel, and alternately advance and retract the lips. The ROUNDED VOWELS rounding of (i) results in (y) (high-front-tense-round), which is the sound of French u in ‘dur,’ ‘but,’ ‘vu,’ etc. This sound, which often presents great difficulties to English people, may often be perfectly acquired in a few minutes by the above simple experiment. The same acoustic effect may be produced by forming a small circle with the finger and thumb, and pronouncing (i) through this, when the effect, if the aperture be sufficiently small, will at once be (y), which, perhaps, the student has long tried in vain to pronounce. It should be noted that the degree of rounding—that is, of the smallness of the aperture—is normally related to the height of the tongue, so that in most languages high vowels have the greatest, and low vowels the least degree of rounding. But languages sometimes develop vowels in which the rounding is abnormal—high vowels with the slighter rounding generally associated with mid or low vowels, or low or mid vowels with a greater amount of rounding than is usual to those degrees of height. In the former case we speak of under-rounding, in the latter we say that the vowel is over-rounded.

Examples of the latter process are found in Swedish long o, mid-back-tense, with over-rounding, which to foreign ears sounds like (ū), and in the German ü, which is the mid-front-tense, with over-rounding, the acoustic effect being identical with that of French (y) to untrained ears. An example of an under-rounded vowel is heard in the Lancashire sound of the vowel in ‘bush,’ ‘butcher, etc. (mid-back-tense, under-rounded).

In describing a vowel, the four points above discussed are mentioned in the order in which we have dealt with them If there be no rounding, it is usually unnecessary to mention the action of the lips, it being assumed that these play no part in the particular sound unless the rounding be stated.

Thus (ū) in ‘boot’ is the high-back-tense-round; the (ā) in ‘father’ the mid-back-slack.

From the above account it will be seen that there are thirty-six main normal vowels: three back, three front, and three flat or mixed vowels, according to the height of the tongue—that is, nine positions; the sounds associated with each of these positions are further increased by another nine, giving eighteen, according to whether the tongue be tense or slack; and, lastly, every tense and every slack vowel may be rounded, bringing the number up to thirty-six.

Shifted Vowels.—Mr. Sweet, in the second edition of his Primer of Phonetics, has recently pointed out that it is possible, while using the back of the tongue, to shift the raised part forward, so that the air-passage is narrowed further forward than in the case of the normal vowels, where the narrowing takes places between the tongue and that part of the palate immediately above the area of activity. Similarly, in articulating front vowels, the tongue may be drawn back, so the area of articulation is further back in the palate, although the front of the tongue is still used. The character of these ‘shifted’ vowels is, according to Mr. Sweet’s view, sufficiently distinct from that of vowels formed in normal manner to justify the former being classified as distinct sounds. This brings the number of well-marked, distinct vowel sounds up to seventy-two. Many of the Modern English dialects contain ‘shifted’ vowels, which it is very difficult to locate, unless this possibility be remembered.

Intermediate Varieties of Vowel Sounds.—It must be MINUTE SHADES OF SOUND borne in mind that the above enumeration and tabulating of vowels according to the Organic System only deals with the chief, distinctive types. Thus (i) (high-front) is quite distinct from (e) (mid-front), both to the ear and to the muscular sense, but it is possible to lower the tongue gradually from the high position to one which produces a sound different from the typical vowel associated with that position, but not yet fully a mid vowel. In such a case we should have to determine whether the position was, as a matter of fact, nearer to the high or the mid. In the former case we should classify the vowel as a high vowel lowered; in the latter, as a mid vowel raised.

These intermediate positions occur in all languages, especially in dialects. In Danish the ordinary (ē) (mid-front) is so far raised towards the high position that the effect it produces upon the ear of a foreigner at the first hearing is almost that of (ī). In many Scotch dialects the high-front-slack vowel is considerably lowered, almost to the position of the mid-front (ɛ), and the mid-front is also lowered almost to (æ). So alike is the Scotch (i) in ‘bit’ to the English (ɛ) in ‘bet’ that, unless the mid-front were also proportionately lowered, the two sounds would be confused. As a rule, language shrinks from having two distinct vowels so closely alike as (i) lowered, and normal (ɛ) at one and the same period—if one is lowered the other is lowered too.

In English there is a tendency, at any rate among speakers of standard English, to avoid these lowered vowels altogether, and to pronounce the normal high and mid vowels. This gives to the standard dialect a certain clearness and distinctness which is often lacking in the pronunciation of other dialects.

Glides.—In ordinary speech the vocal organs, especially the tongue, frequently have to assume, in rapid succession, a series of positions which are very different, and comparatively far removed one from the other, as one sound after another is uttered by the speaker. To get from one position to another, the organs move with great rapidity, and these movements are called glides. It sometimes happens that the passage of the organs from one position to another results in audible sounds. The sounds are called glide sounds, and sometimes also, merely glides.

We may distinguish: (1) Glides produced as the organs pass from repose to activity—that is, when beginning to speak; (2) those due to the organs passing from one mode of activity to another—these occur during the utterance of words or word-series; (3) the movements of the organs in passing from a state of activity to one of repose—that is, when pausing or ceasing to speak.

Glides are very important to the student of language, for they not only are very characteristic of any actually spoken language, but in the history of a language they often develop into independent sounds.

To illustrate these two points. It makes all the difference to the pronunciation of French whether a foreigner, especially an Englishman, has acquired the proper glides after the voiceless stops, p, t, k. In French, when these sounds are followed by a vowel, the voicing begins before the stop is opened, so that the latter part of the consonant is rarely voiced. In English and German, on the other hand, after voiceless stops, the vocal chords are not closed until the stops have been opened, so that there is a slight GLIDES puff of breath between the stop and the following vowel. A glide after a sound is called an Off-glide, so that we say that in French there is a Voice off-glide after voiceless stops, but in English a Breath off-glide. To show how important glides are in the development of language, we may instance the process known as Fracture, or Brechung, in O.E. In primitive O.E. such a form as *œld (‘old’) became *œuld in the South, by the development of the glide between the front vowel œ and the following -ld into a full vowel. This primitive œu subsequently became œa, written ea, in eald from *œld, beald from *bœld, etc. The other Germanic languages and some of the English dialects developed no vowel from the off-glide in these cases, so that at the present day we have old from an Anglian āld (late Anglian), and in High German alt.

The whole subject of glides demands the special attention of the student, and he must study the phenomena in his own speech, aided by the special phonetic treatises; but enough has, perhaps, been said here to make the term and the ideas connected with it intelligible in subsequent references in the present work.

Accent.

Under this head are often included two quite distinct phenomena—Stress or Emphasis, and Intonation.

Stress depends upon the degree of force with which the air-stream is expelled from the lungs. An increase of force in the air-stream causes increased loudness in the case of vowels and all voiced sounds.

We distinguish three chief degrees of stress—Strong, Medium, Weak. These terms are, of course, purely relative. When a word consists of several syllables, various degrees of stress are exhibited in its pronunciation. Thus in such a word as ‘perceptible,’ the strongest stress is on the second syllable, the weakest on the first, the next weakest on the third, and the second strongest on the fourth. The tendency is to alternate strong and weak stress. When we speak of the stressed syllable of a word, we mean the syllable which has the chief, or strongest, stress. When we say that a syllable is unstressed we mean that it has the weakest stress: some force it must have, otherwise it would be inaudible, and would disappear altogether. The disappearance of very weakly stressed syllables is a frequent phenomenon in the history of language. In Modern English certain words are differently stressed, according to the sentence in which they occur. Thus the auxiliary ‘have’ occurs in the forms (hæv) with strong stress, (həv) with weaker stress, (v) when completely unstressed. Compare the sentences: (wɛə hǽv ju bīn? wɛ́ər (h)əv ju bīn? ai v bīn in landən).

As regards the distribution of stress, we can distinguish three varieties—Increasing, Even, and Diminishing stress. In English the highest point of stress in an emphatic syllable is the beginning, from which point the force in a monosyllabic word is diminished. In the distribution of stress over a word of several syllables, or over a breath-group—that is, the whole series of syllables uttered with one breath—the force is usually varied during the utterance by alternately increasing and diminishing the air-stream.

Even stress implies that the degree of force is maintained constant throughout the utterance. This never actually ACCENT—QUANTITY happens in English, since in the single syllable the stress is decreased so that it gets weaker and weaker, and if, as happens comparatively rarely, two succeeding syllables have an equal amount of stress, the second is uttered with a fresh impulse of the breath, as in plum cake (plám kɛ̄ik), John Jones (džón džóunz).

Stress is an important factor in determining syllable division.

Intonation is a question of pitch. Alterations of pitch in speech are produced by tightening the vocal chords for a high tone, loosening or shortening them for a low tone.

We have Rising Intonation, as in the interrogative, sharply-uttered ‘what?’ Falling Intonation, as in the negative reply to a question—‘no!’ Fall and Rise is heard in the warning or expostulatory ‘take care!’ uttered with a certain impatience; Rise and Fall in the contemptuous or supercilious ‘oh!’ These combined tones are of importance in the history of language, but they cannot easily be studied except with the aid of oral instruction.

It should be noted that every speaker naturally pitches his voice on a certain note as his normal pitch; every tone which he utters above this is a rise, every one below it is a fall. The degree of rise and fall which takes place in speech is different in, and very characteristic of, different languages or dialects.

Quantity.—This, again, is a relative term; long vowels in some languages are shorter than in others. Differences of quantity exist in consonants also. In English, final voiced consonants are long compared to those of German. Contrast, for instance, the final n of English ‘man,’ and German ‘mann.’

It is important to distinguish between a long and a double consonant. The latter class are heard in Swedish, Italian, and many other languages. They even occur in English in such compounds as ‘book-case.’ In a double consonant the position of the vocal organs is maintained for a certain space of time, and a new impulse of breath is given in the middle, whereas in a long consonant there is no fresh impulse of breath during the maintenance of the position. A further possibility is to utter the same consonant twice—that is, with two off-glides. This is occasionally heard from very self-conscious and affected speakers in English, who are trying to ‘talk fine.’ ‘This hill has a flat top’ would normally be pronounced (ðis hil hæz ə flættɔp), with no escape of breath between the t of flat and that of top; the affected pronunciation referred to would be (flæt tɔp), with an off-glide after each t, before the new impulse of breath. It is to be observed that there is no necessary connection between the quantity and the quality of vowels; that is to say, that any vowel may be pronounced either long or short. In English tense (i) only occurs long, but in French it is usually quite short. Again, the mid-front-slack (ɛ) is always short in English at the present time in the standard language, but many of the dialects have (ɛ̄), which is also common in French, as in ‘bête’ (bɛ̄t), etc.

Syllable Division.—The essential characteristic of a syllable is that there is no sense of break or interruption to destroy its unity. Anything which causes a break in continuity produces a sense of duality, and tends to SYLLABLE DIVISION destroy the unity of the syllable.

The interruption of the unity of a syllable may be caused in various ways:

1. By alternation of strong and weak stress. So long as the stress is even or gradually diminishing, a vowel may be prolonged indefinitely without producing upon the ear the sense of discontinuity. But if we pronounce a very long vowel, such as (ā), and alternately increase and diminish the stress, we at once break it up into as many syllables as there are increases and decreases: (á-a-á-a-á-a), and so on.

2. By alternating greater and lesser sonority. The vowel (a) is more sonorous than (i), because the mouth passage is wider when pronouncing it, and consequently a bigger volume of voice can pass through. If, therefore, we alternate (a-i-a-i-a)—that is, first strong, then weak, then strong sonority—we cannot escape the sense of as many syllables as there are increases after reductions of sonority.

In a true diphthong, such as (ai), as in English ‘bite,’ we have, it is true, a gradual reduction of sonority and of stress; but the sense of unity is not lost, because the reduction is so gradual, and because the second vowel loses its syllabicness by virtue of its lack of sonority as compared with the preceding (a), which also bears the stress. A true diphthong may be defined as a combination of two vowels, of which only one is syllabic, the other having neither stress nor sonority in comparison, and being therefore non-syllabic.

3. The interruption of continuity may be produced by the air-stream being either very considerably hindered, through the narrowing of the mouth passage, as by an Open Consonant, or altogether checked for a moment, as by a Stop Consonant. The presence of a consonant between two vowels, since it breaks the continuity more or less completely, must of necessity produce two syllables.

The Limits of the Syllable.[B]—A syllable ends when the weakest degree of stress is reached, and the next begins with the fresh increase. Thus in England we pronounce the name of the famous University and golfing city of Fifeshire, St. Andrews, as (sənt ǽndrūz), but in Scotland itself the universal pronunciation is (sən tandrūz); that is, we continue to diminish the stress until the off-glide of the t, whereas the Scotch reach their weakest stress with the n.


For a clear and admirable treatment of Quantity, Syllable Division, Stress, and Intonation, cf. Jespersen, Lehrbuch der Phonetik, 1904, pp. 173-240.

Phonetic Symbols.

A few remarks upon the use of a phonetic transcription will not be out of place here.

The Organic symbols are, of course, by far the most accurate, since they are not mere arbitrary alphabetic signs, but are intended to express the actual positions of the organs of speech, the presence or absence of breath, of rounding, of nasality, and so on. But it is admitted that they are cumbersome, and for the transcription of words and sentences a simpler notation can be used with advantage. Sweet’s Broad Romic is a convenient system of symbols which is widely used, and the International alphabet is employed by Passy, Lloyd, Vietor, and many other phoneticians.

After all, any alphabet is a mere convention, and provided we know what sounds we intend to express, the simpler the method of graphic expression the better. In USE OF PHONETIC SYMBOLS dealing with a single language, or a limited series of sounds, it is best first to define in the terminology of the organic system the value of the symbols commonly employed in the ordinary spelling of the language in question, and then to adopt some familiar symbol to express the sound whenever it occurs. Thus, if we know that French u in ‘but,’ ‘vu,’ etc., is the high-front-tense-round, we may use any recognised symbol we choose to express it, provided our employment of the symbol be consistent. Thus ü, y would both serve the purpose. If we have defined ü or y as = high-front-tense-round when transcribing French, there is no reason why the same symbol should not be used to express a different sound in our transcription of another language which does not possess h-f-t-r. In Russian, for instance, it is often convenient to use y for the high-flat-tense, since in that language h-f-t-r does not occur.

This economic principle of using the same symbol for different sounds in different languages has the advantage of avoiding the inconvenience of mastering seventy-two perfectly arbitrary symbols for the vowels, many of which we may never need at all. In oral teaching, when demonstrating on the blackboard, and in scientific treatises, Sweet’s organic symbols for the vowels are exceedingly convenient, since they are easily mastered and are perfectly definite in significance. It is useful when writing to be able to express with a single symbol such facts as the exact position of the tongue and lips, thus conveying precisely the shade of sound which we are dealing with. Otherwise we must, in exact discussion, use the cumbersome ‘high-front-tense-round,’ which we may, however, shorten as above to h-f-t-r, and so on with all the other vowels.

The symbol ⊤, really a pointer indicating direction, is useful in conjunction with alphabetic signs. ⊤ means lowering of the tongue, ⊥ raising, ⊢ advancing, and ⊣ retraction. Thus if (ɛ) be the symbol for the normal mid-front-slack, (ɛ ⊤) would indicate the lowered Scotch variety.

Tables of Phonetic Symbols for Consonants and Vowels used in this Book.

 

THE CONSONANTS.

 

[B = Breath, V = Voice]

Back.Front.Blade.Blade-point.Point.
BVBVBVBVBV
Openhʓjszʃžþð
Stopkgċtd
Nasalŋn
Dividedɫl
Lip.Lip-teeth.Lip-back.
BVBVBV
Openfvw
Stoppb
Nasalm
Divided

Table of Vowel Symbols and their Values.

SLACK VOWELS.

Unround.

Front.Back.Flat.
Highi, Eng. bit
Midɛ, Eng. heada{ Gm. hat
{ Eng. fatherə, father
Lowæ, Eng. cat

Round.

Front.Back.Flat.
Highu, Eng. put
Mido, Gm. stock
Lowɔ, Eng. hot

TENSE VOWELS.

Unround.

Front.Back.Flat.
Highi{ Fr. si }(short)ï, Scot. dial, buik
{ Eng. see}(long)
Mide,Fr. a, Eng. but
Lowʌ, Eng. bird

Round.

Front.Back.Flat.
Highy, Fr. luneu{ Eng. boot }(long)
{ Gm. blume }
Scot. put (short)
Mido{ Gm. lohn} (long)
{ Fr. beau} (shorter)
Lowɔ,Eng. saw

In order not to multiply symbols beyond what is absolutely necessary, (h) will be used initially in phonetic transcription to express the ordinary ‘aspirate’ of Modern English; medially and finally it indicates a back-open-voiceless consonant. (r) is not included in the above table; English r in the South is a weak point-teeth-open consonant, in Scotch it is a point-trill, in French a back-trill. In some of the English dialects of the South and Midlands it is an inverted consonant—i.e., an open consonant formed by the point of the tongue turned upwards and backwards.

ċ, ġ are habitually written at the present day in the ordinary spelling of O.E. to indicate fronted sounds; the latter is generally pronounced as a front-open consonant in O.E., as in ġiefan, ‘give.’ When used in the special way indicated above, all symbols are in this book enclosed in brackets; thus ġiefan would be (jievan), etc.

Length is marked by a stroke above the letter—ā, ʌ̄, etc. A vowel symbol which is not thus marked is intended to express a short sound, and shortness is otherwise not specially indicated as a rule. The symbol ˜ placed over a vowel implies nasalization, as in Fr. (kɔ̃tã) content.

Forms placed in brackets are intended to express the pronunciation, according to the above table of symbols. The ordinary spelling is either in italics or in inverted commas—e.g., ‘hot’ (hɔt), ‘father’ (fāðə).

It will be observed that the slack vowels are represented by italic letters, except in the cases of (ɛ), (ə), and (æ), which are well known, and convenient; the symbols for the tense vowels are all romic. Italic letters, therefore, enclosed in brackets always indicate slack, and romic always tense vowels.


CHAPTER III

HOW LANGUAGE IS ACQUIRED AND HANDED ON

One of the most familiar incidents of daily life is that of a child learning to speak. It is an experience which every normal human being has undergone in his own person, although the memory of the first steps is lost long before the process is nearly complete. The infant slowly learns to utter a few intelligible sounds in his native tongue from those who surround him—his parents, his nurse, his brothers and sisters. He learns by imitation to reproduce, at first very imperfectly, the sounds which he hears, and by constant repetition on the part of his first teachers, accompanied by explanatory gestures, such as pointing to a person or a thing, or performing an action while uttering its name, he gradually comes to connect the uttered sound with the person, the object, or the action which it symbolizes.

Those who in after-life acquire a foreign language in the country itself, or among native speakers, nurses, governesses, etc., in their own country, to a certain extent repeat the process whereby they originally learnt their own language. This is undoubtedly the most direct and natural way of mastering a language, and, supplemented later on by the artificial aids of grammar and dictionary, it gives a grip of the genius of a foreign tongue, and forms the speech instinct in a way that no other method can accomplish. It is a remarkable fact, when we reflect upon the difficulties which in later life beset the learning of a new language, especially the new pronunciation, that within a few years the child acquires with perfect exactness, in all normal cases, the pronunciation of those speakers from whom he learns his native language. Of course, there are cases of inherent defective utterance, in which certain sounds remain difficult or even impossible to pronounce perfectly to the end of the life of the speaker. It is also true, as we shall see, that no two speakers of the same community or the same family do, in all respects, pronounce exactly alike. Still, the fact remains that after a few years the child can and does, to all intents and purposes, reproduce the pronunciation of the circle in which he is brought up, with so great a degree of fidelity, that his pronunciation is felt by everyone to be identical with that upon which it is based—the speech of his family and closest intimates. It would appear that this power of learning by imitation pure and simple is, as a rule, limited to the sounds of the mother-tongue, or at most one or two other languages which are acquired in early childhood.

To understand the reason of this we must inquire more closely what are the processes which actually come into play in the utterance of speech sounds.

First of all the organs of speech perform certain movements, in order to get into the position necessary for the production of the sound to be uttered. This series of movements, and this position, which is maintained for a certain time, gives rise to characteristic muscular sensations. MEMORY-PICTURES OF SOUND AND POSITION Then the sound is uttered, and this, again, produces a definite physical sensation upon the auditory nerves. These muscular sensations and this auditory experience are the physiological processes involved in each utterance of a sound. But this is not all; each nervous impression is recorded in the consciousness, and goes to form what may be called memory-pictures. In the utterance of a speech sound memory-pictures are formed—(a) of the sound itself, (b) of the muscular sensations arising from the movements of the vocal organs into the required position, and of a certain characteristic tension required to maintain the position during the utterance of the sound. That is to say, that in addition to the memory-picture of sound, there are also formed memory-pictures of the movement series and of the position. These memory-pictures of sound, movements, and position, are the psychological processes which accompany the utterance of every speech sound. These memory-pictures are formed unconsciously, but until they are formed it is impossible to reproduce a speech sound. This is why a child only slowly acquires the power to reproduce the sounds of his mother-tongue. The first mental picture formed is that of the sound itself, as heard from others. Then there is a tentative groping to reproduce it, but the necessary series of organic movements, and the position, have generally to be learnt, as the results of many mistaken attempts. Thus, when a child substitutes a point-stop (t) for a back-stop (k), and says, for instance, (tis) for (kis), it is probable that he can discriminate between the two sounds when he hears them; but his inability to do so in his own speech is due to the fact that he has not yet learned to form a stop with the back of his tongue, although he can do so with the point. The movement of retracting the tongue, and the position of the tongue pressed against the soft palate are unfamiliar, and have to be acquired by experiment. When once the unaccustomed movements have been performed, a faint mental picture is recorded, which makes the next utterance easier. With each repeated carrying out of a series of movements the memory-picture becomes clearer and more definite, until at last, the series being faithfully and definitely imprinted upon the memory, it can be reproduced accurately at will. The memory-picture of the sound is often more distinct, because the sound is heard not only from our own pronunciation, in which it gradually becomes associated with those of the movements and position, but also frequently in the pronunciation of others. Whereas, then, the sound-picture is made stronger by hearing other speakers, the movement and position pictures can only be made clearer by our own pronunciation of the sound. The sound-picture sometimes remains clear when the position-picture has become blurred, and faint from lack of habit in uttering the sound, in which case the former helps to correct and reconstruct the latter, because the result of our attempts at pronunciation does not satisfy our recollection of the sound.

It may be noted here that it is important not to allow those who are learning a foreign language to get into the habit of wrong pronunciation; since each repeated utterance of the wrong sound makes the memory-picture of the movements and position clearer and deeper, and therefore increasingly difficult to eradicate. Teachers who trust to imitation alone in imparting a foreign pronunciation, FORMATION OF SPEECH HABITS often repeat the desired sound hundreds of times with little result, the reason being that while the pupil’s correct sound-picture may indeed be strengthened, the wrong position-picture remains uncorrected, and becomes clearer and more imperishable each time the same mistake in pronunciation is made. Thus a discrepancy often arises between the memory-picture of the sound and that of the process of reproducing it. It is this existence of the memory-pictures of the sounds and positions which occur in our own language, and which we have strengthened for years by daily habit, that makes it so difficult to form fresh memory-pictures in later life. Our speech habit has become inveterate, and we cannot easily acquire a different one.

With the young child the case is different. His mental and bodily habits are of recent formation, his speech basis is not fixed; he can easily change it, or form a new set of memory-pictures, both of sounds and of physical movements: hence he can more readily acquire the sounds of a foreign language than the adult.

The complex processes of utterance, even those involved in producing the sounds of our mother-tongue, are for the most part quite unrealized by the speaker. The series of memory-pictures graven upon the consciousness give rise to the familiar series of movements and positions, and to the sounds associated with them, and yet we are unaware both of the psychological and of the physiological part of the process. A phonetic training involves learning to realize and recognise both of these aspects of utterance. We have to bring the mental pictures and the resultant movements and positions from the plane of unconsciousness or subconsciousness to that of full consciousness. Most people, as soon as they think about the subject, can realize mentally, the series of movements which are necessary to the pronunciation of many of the familiar consonants, such as p, t, and even k, though this is more difficult, without (even silently) going through the actual movements themselves. But most untrained experimenters will probably find, at first, that they are unable to realize at all, the series of movements required for the pronunciation of even such familiar vowel sounds as (ī), as in ‘bee’ (bī), or ɔ̄, as in ‘saw’ (ɔ̄). To assist in bringing the familiar but unrealized processes of pronunciation into the realms of definite consciousness, the beginner may be recommended to pronounce some familiar sound aloud several times, concentrating his attention upon the movements which the vocal organs instinctively perform; then to ‘whisper’ the sound, still closely observing the movements; then to go through the series of movements silently, not even uttering the sound in a ‘whisper’; and finally to reproduce the series mentally, without carrying out the movements at all. It will be seen that such an exercise can only be carried out with sounds which are perfectly familiar, and which the vocal organs can produce instinctively through the existence of a clear (although subconscious) memory-picture. It follows that the necessary and proper basis for phonetic training is the careful study of the mother-tongue, and of that particular form of it which we naturally and habitually use. Thus it would be an unsound method for a dialect speaker, or one whose pronunciation was strongly coloured by a ‘provincial accent,’ to begin the scientific study or sounds by THE PHONETIC CONSCIENCE considering first of all the sounds of some ideal ‘standard’ of English speech which were quite unfamiliar, and which he would almost certainly not reproduce accurately. This is especially true of Scotch speakers, who, even if they do not speak ‘broad Scotch,’ have in nearly all cases a strongly-marked Scotch speech basis, for which there are, of course, good historical reasons. It cannot be too strongly insisted upon that the student must cultivate a ‘phonetic conscience,’ and study the sounds of his own natural speech as they are, without attempting to change them or ‘fake’ them in any way. They are the only sounds which he is an absolute master of, which he makes instinctively and without taking thought, and they are therefore the only sounds upon which he can properly begin his observations. When he is able to analyze the mental and physical processes involved in his own natural pronunciation, the student can proceed, being now a master of the power of analysis, and having gained some conscious control of his vocal organs, to practise new series of movements, and thus to acquire new sounds.

From the above considerations, the reason for our reiterated insistence upon the importance of our own form of speech as the basis of scientific linguistic study will, perhaps, become more apparent. Anyone who has gone through the somewhat difficult mill of systematic linguistic training can but smile at the arguments adduced against beginning with the native dialect by those who are completely innocent of any real knowledge of what is aimed at, or of the methods whereby it alone can be achieved.

The fact that the processes of speech utterance are naturally unconscious is an important one, in view of the bearing which, as we shall see hereafter, it has upon the question of sound change. This fact can readily be ascertained by any beginner who tries to realize mentally, in the manner suggested above, how he produces any vowel sound which is familiar to him in his own pronunciation of English. Such an attempt will at once bring the truth of the foregoing statement home to the student in the most convincing manner. It is, however, just one of those essential general principles, an ignorance of which renders unreal and fruitless any discussion of the important question of sound change, and of the closely allied conception of phonetic law.

It is probably the too exclusive study of the literary form of language which fosters the view, so often taught, or at least implied in the teaching given, that speech is deliberate and conscious, and that the speaker, even when talking naturally and untrammelled by conventional models, definitely intends to pronounce in a certain way, which he elects to use rather than another.

In writing, the whole process is fraught with a certain deliberation, which is encouraged by the necessity of paying attention to the formation of the letters and the correct spelling, although even this becomes largely instinctive by long habit. There is in writing, however, a constant attention to literary form, a deliberate selection of words and forms of sentence, which takes place here to a far greater extent than is possible in any but the most studied kind of public discourse, and which is almost entirely absent from familiar and colloquial speech.

At any rate, it is certain that the natural speaker is quite unconscious even of the precise acoustic effect of the SPEECH ENVIRONMENT sounds which he uses, while of the subtle and delicate adjustments and co-ordinations of the vocal mechanism he is completely ignorant. He does not attempt, consciously at least, either to preserve or to modify any sound or syllable.

The pronunciation of other speakers, which we may call the ‘speech environment,’ certainly exercises an influence upon every individual. From others he learned his pronunciation to start with, and from those with whom he is brought in contact throughout his life he, in a sense, goes on learning so long as his sense of hearing lasts:—that is to say, the speech of the individual tends to approximate to the average speech of those with whom he is brought into contact. This influence of one speaker upon another, which will be discussed more at length in another chapter, is, however, normally, unperceived by those who undergo it.

The case in which a speaker, from Scotland, let us say, comes to England, and definitely and deliberately tries to get rid of his ‘Scotch accent,’ and adopts the speech of the South, is nothing against the general principle that the influence of one form of speech upon another is exerted unconsciously. In the case cited we have, to start with, a conventional and artificial preference for Southern rather than for Northern English, and, further, what takes place is simply that the speaker chooses to learn another dialect. This differs only in degree from the case in which a Dutchman in Germany elects to acquire and to speak German.

If it be true that the language of every speaker undergoes, throughout his life, a continuous influence from other speakers with whom he comes in contact, it would seem as though the process of ‘acquiring’ a language was one which is never complete, and which never ceases while life and intelligence remain. And this is, in a sense, the case; but it is possible and useful to set a limit in thought to the period during which the native language is being acquired. Certainly, as far as pronunciation is concerned, we may say that, up to a point, the child is still ‘learning’ to speak. There comes a time, however, when he has mastered all the sounds in use among those with whom he lives. Those with whom he associates most closely during this early period of life, may be considered as his ‘speech parents’—those from whom he learns. After this the circle of persons with whom he comes in contact will, in all probability, be greatly widened with advancing years. The unconscious influence of this growing circle of speakers affects his pronunciation; but less and less so after the early years, for the reason that the individual has already ‘learnt’ his language, has formed his own speech basis, and has an independent existence as a speaker. Therefore the unconscious influence of other speakers upon the pronunciation of an individual acts slowly, and is comparatively slight after this first period. As regards the other sides of language, vocabulary and sentence-structure, these are undoubtedly susceptible of unconscious modification for a very much longer period. These aspects of language are the expression of personal culture and experience, and naturally tend to become richer, more complex and more varied, with the growth of the intellectual and moral man.

The life-history of the speech of the individual is a part of the history of the language; and so, the problem of the LANGUAGE CHANGED IN TRANSMISSION acquirement of his language by the individual, is part of the general problem of the development of language.

For we cannot regard language as something which is handed on in a fixed and definite form from one individual, and acquired in precisely the same form by another. It is changed, however inconsiderably, in the very process of transmission, re-minted at the outset by the crucible of the new mind into which it passes, and the slightly different physical organism, which performs afresh the movements of speech.

Thus we see that the elements of change in language lie in the transmission from one generation to another, and in the essential differences which exist between individuals.

The conception of an absolutely uniform language, existing even during a single generation, and in a single small community, is in reality a mere hypothetical assumption.

We shall now have to consider how far uniformity of speech actually does exist, in what way definite tendencies of change arise in the individual, why and to what extent these are shared by the community at large.


Note.—In pursuing the study of the General Principles of the development of language, which are dealt with in this and several subsequent chapters of this book, the student should consult:

Sweet: Words, Logic, and Grammar, Trans. Phil. Soc., 1875-1876. History of Language, Dent, 1900. History of English Sounds, §§ 1-241, Oxford, 1888.

Strong, Logemann, and Wheeler: History of Language, Longmans, 1891.

Paul: Principien der Sprachgeschichte.

[An epoch-making book; has contributed largely to form the modern point of view. Most writers on General Principles at the present day draw their inspiration primarily from it.]

Wechssler: Gibt es Lautgesetze? 1900.

Osthoff and Brugmann: Vorwort to Morphologische Untersuchungen, Erster Theil, 1878.

Other works will be referred to in the course of the following pages. My debt to all the above is very great—I acknowledge it here—for the general treatment of the subjects discussed in the next few chapters.


CHAPTER IV

SOUND CHANGE

By the phrase ‘sound change’ is meant those changes in pronunciation which take place in every language in the course of time. It is easy to convince ourselves that changes of pronunciation have occurred in English, for instance, in the last 200 years. Pope’s lines—

‘And praise the easy vigour of a line,

Where Denham’s strength, and Waller’s sweetness join’

—are often quoted to illustrate the fact, borne out by other evidence, that the rhymes in his time were (ləin—džəin).

Again, the same poet writes:

‘Fearing ev’n fools, by flatterers besieged,

And so obliging, that he ne’er obliged,’

where the last word was undoubtedly pronounced (ōblīdžd). These rhymes at least illustrate the fact that less than 200 years ago two English words were pronounced by a cultivated person like Pope, who frequented the best English society of his day, in a manner which at the present time would strike people of the same standing as strange, if not vulgar.

If we consider the written records of still earlier periods of our language in the light of that method of interpreting the old symbols which we owe primarily to the late Mr. A. J. Ellis, the differences of pronunciation which we are able to feel certain existed between the speech of these periods and that of the present day are so great that, putting aside the other differences of vocabulary and the general structure of the language, we cannot doubt that the English of King Alfred, of Chaucer, and even of Shakespeare, would be largely unintelligible to us, if we were able to ‘hold an hour’s communion with the dead.’

If this remarkable amount of change has taken place in a few centuries in the pronunciation of several generations of Englishmen living in England, how much greater will be the degree of change which the pronunciation of one and the same language will undergo in the course of several thousands of years among separate nations living in widely remote countries! We can form some idea of the possibilities of the extent of divergence from an original form under these conditions if we consider the diversity which the same word exhibits in the various Aryan families of speech.

It might seem at the first blush improbable or impossible that Scrt. dhūmas, Gk. θῡ́μος, Lat. fūmus, O.Sl. dȳmŭ, Gothic dauns, O.E. dū-st, from earlier *dunst (Eng. dust), can have anything in common as regards form, and yet, unless the modern science of Comparative Philology is entirely vain and its methods futile, all these words are merely the various pronunciations, developed in the course of long ages, of the same original word or ‘root’ among different branches of Aryan speech. In the case of the O.E. word dūst there is also a difference of suffix; Lat., Gk., Scrt. and O.Sl. agree in having an original long ū compared with a short, but also original vowel in the other languages; while the Gothic dauns has, again, a different, but equally original, form of the MODIFICATION OF THE MEMORY-PICTURES vowel; otherwise the above forms are completely cognate.

It is proposed in this chapter to discuss how, and from what cause, the sounds of speech undergo change.

And first let us say that, although the phrase ‘sound change’ is convenient and in universal use, it is, from the point of view of strict accuracy, erroneous. For we are to consider that a sound in itself cannot change; it is uttered and is gone: it has in itself no permanence. When we say that the same sound is repeated, we mean that an identical, or nearly identical, series of movements of the vocal organs is performed, and that the same acoustic effect is produced as upon a former occasion.

The permanent element in uttered speech—that part, therefore, which is capable of a historical development—is the psychological element, those groups of memory-pictures upon which we dwelt in the preceding chapter.

The pronunciation of the same word in the same community is different from one age to another; we say, speaking loosely, that in this case the sounds of the community have changed. What has really happened is that the underlying memory-pictures of sound and movements undergo gradual modification, and are different in one age from what they were in a former, and, in all probability, from what they will be later on.

If this is borne in mind, we may continue to speak of ‘sound change,’ meaning thereby a change in the aggregate of mental pictures possessed by all the individuals of a community, the result of which is that a series of substitutions takes place of one sound for another, until the sounds actually pronounced by a later generation in the same word differ widely from those pronounced by an earlier generation (cf. Wechssler, pp. 26, 27).

If the pronunciation of a language changes, it can only be due to the fact that the vocal organs are used by the members of a community in a different way at one period from what they are at another; the series of movements of the vocal organs, the positions which these assume in speaking, and therefore the underlying mental pictures of these, have been modified.

We have said that that group of physical movements and those underlying groups of mental pictures which exist at any moment among the members of a community constitute what is known as the ‘speech basis.’

An inquiry into the causes and processes of sound change, then, is actually an inquiry into the conditions under which the speech basis of a community is gradually modified.

It will be convenient to consider the question, in the first instance, as it affects the individual, since the speech of a community is obviously merely the collective utterance of the individuals of which it is composed. The relation of the individual to his community will be discussed in the next chapter.

All bodily movements which are the result of volition can only be carried out by virtue of the subconscious memory-picture which they reproduce each time the action is repeated. Until this memory-picture is formed, the series of movements is uncertain and imperfect. If we take the case of such a highly-specialized series of co-ordinated movements as those necessary to ‘cast a fly’ in fishing, or of using a billiard cue so as to produce a ‘screw,’ it is evident that these, like the series of movements of the vocal organs which produce a speech sound, LIMITS OF UNPERCEIVED DEVIATION can only be successfully carried out as the result of considerable practice. In all cases the memory-picture must be clear and definite. Now, it is evident that although a practised fisherman can generally throw a fly so as to produce approximately the desired result—in this case, that is to say, to put it modestly, at least in such a way as not to flick the fly off—he nevertheless does not reproduce in each successive cast precisely and absolutely the same series of movements; there are variations in the degree of force, in the direction, in the curves described by the hand as it is raised and brought forward again after the line has been straightened behind the fisherman, and in many other ways too subtle to analyze. Yet each successful cast (successful in the sense indicated above) satisfies the person who performs the movements: he feels that he has cast his fly in the proper way. This merely means that, in spite of divergence, the series of movements corresponds to, and reproduces the memory-picture of the process sufficiently exactly for the divergence not to be appreciable. A certain possible limit of deviation from the memory-picture exists, within which the departure is unperceived. If, however, the divergence of the action from the memory-picture of this be too great, the fisherman is conscious of it, and feels that he has made a bad throw a fact of which the loss of his fly probably adds further confirmation.

In just the same way, the actions of the vocal organs in speech, reproduce the memory-pictures approximately, though not always exactly. Here, again, if the movement-series deviates beyond a certain extent from the mental picture, the divergence is recognised, partly by the actual muscular sensation, but more generally by reason of the divergence of the result from the memory-picture of the sound.

But the memory-pictures themselves are not homogeneous, and composed of only one kind of impression; for each repeated utterance of the sound leaves its trace upon the mental picture. Upon the mind is recorded each divergence from the original picture—that is, a new impression of a slightly different character is made. Of the various impressions recorded, the most recent are the deepest and most potent; so that in the course of time the new impressions outweigh the older in the memory-picture. Thus in time the aggregate of impressions result in a memory-picture which is of a slightly different character from the old one. From this new memory-picture the same degree of unperceived divergence is possible, this degree being always constant; but since the memory-picture itself has been modified, the starting-point of divergence has also been shifted slightly further from the original point of departure.

To put the matter in another way, if the change in pronunciation is sufficiently gradual, if it does not proceed further than a certain point at a time, the individual does not perceive the slight shifting which has taken place, and the impression is unconsciously recorded. If, however, the pronunciation at a given moment of utterance is too far from what the speaker instinctively feels to be the normal, he at once perceives the difference, and ‘corrects’ the result as a ‘mistake’ or a ‘slip of the tongue.’ Thus, on account of the inherent instability of the organs of speech and the habits of using them, the pronunciation EXAMPLES OF ISOLATIVE SOUND CHANGE of each individual is continually liable to slight variation, and therefore, gradually, to permanent alteration.

Variation in the speech of the individual is, according to the above statements, in the natural and inevitable order of things. The speech basis is gradually modified, and with it the sounds change.

This natural shifting of the speech basis is the cause of all change in sound, when this is gradual and regular.

Sound changes are conveniently divided into two main classes: Isolative Changes, which take place independent of other neighbouring sounds in the word or sentence, and uninfluenced by them; and Combinative Changes, in which sounds are modified by others which occur in close proximity to them. Both classes of changes depend upon the shifting of the organic basis of speech. It may be well to give at once concrete examples from our own language of each kind of change.

Isolative Changes.—Down to the end of the fifteenth century, or the beginning of the sixteenth, the long sound (ū), whether inherited from Old English or acquired (in French words) during the Middle English period, persisted, so far as we can tell, practically unaltered, unless, indeed, it was shortened by other combinative factors. About the date above mentioned, however, in the South, and far North into the Midlands, (ū) was gradually diphthongized by a process which we need not now discuss, until it reached, probably by the middle of the eighteenth century, its present sound of (au), as in ‘house’ (haus), ‘ground’ (graund), etc. Another isolative change of comparatively recent origin is that of the eighteenth-century (ǣ) sounds to (ā). Almost all (ā) sounds which occur in Modern English, as in ‘father’ (fāðə), ‘rather’ (rāðə), ‘clerk’ (klāk), go back to eighteenth-century (ǣ) sounds, the forms of these words in that century being (fǣðər, rǣðər, klǣrk). This change involves a gradual retraction of the tongue from a low-front vowel position to that of the low-back, which has been subsequently raised, nearly everywhere, to the mid-back, the present sound. It is curious to reflect that during part of the eighteenth century the sound (ā) did not exist in the standard dialect of English. Foreign words, introduced during this period, which contained (ā) in the language from which they were borrowed, still retain the sound (ɔ̄), which was then substituted for the original (ā); thus ‘brandy pawnee’ = (pɔ̄ni), Scrt. pāni, ‘water’; and the place-names Cabul (Kɔ̄bul) for Kābul, and Cawnpore (Kɔ̄́npɔ̄́[ə]). In the same way the now slightly vulgar pronunciation (vɔ̄z) ‘vase’ represents, no doubt, an eighteenth-century attempt at the French sound (vāz).

An old-fashioned pronunciation of ‘rather’ as (reiðə), which still obtains in America, and, curiously enough, in this country also, amongst schoolboys, though only as form of peculiar emphasis, goes back to a different type, eighteenth-century (rēðər), which can be shown to have existed side by side with the type (rǣðər). This form must be still further derived from a M.E. type, rāðer (rāðer), whereas our modern form (rāðə) is from a M.E. răðer, the first vowel of which was fronted to (æ̆) giving (ræðər) in the sixteenth, and (rǣðər), with vowel-lengthening before (ð), in the seventeenth or early eighteenth century. With the exception of this combinative lengthening, all the changes which the two COMBINATIVE FRONTING IN O.E. M.E. types raðer and rāðer have undergone are isolative in character.

Combinative Changes.—The number of these in the history of English, as, indeed, in that of most languages, is very large. A few examples will suffice for the moment.

The two words ‘cold’ and ‘chill’ are both derived from the same root (although they have different suffixes), but different combinative factors have determined their respective forms.

In O.E. these words appear as cāld, an Anglian form, and ċiele, a West Saxon form. It is the difference of the initial with which we are primarily concerned here. In ‘cold,’ from O.E. cāld, from Gmc. *kalda-, the initial consonant, a voiceless back-stop, is the original consonant, and has undergone no change, being followed by a back vowel; in ‘chill,’ however, the O.E. ċiele presupposes an earlier, primitive Old West Saxon *ċeali, from a still earlier *kœli, which comes from a Gmc. *kalī-. In this case the original Gmc. back-stop has been fronted in West Saxon to a front-stop, which has developed into the Modern English ‘ch-’ (tʃ) sound. This is an example of the fact that in prehistoric O.E. a back-stop was fronted to a front-stop before a following front vowel—in this case (æ) low-front. Wherever in Modern English what is popularly called the ‘ch-’ sound (tʃ) occurs in words of native English origin, it is derived from an earlier k, fronted, during the O.E. period, through the influence of a following original front vowel,—one that is, which was already front in the oldest English period.

Other examples of this combinative fronting of an earlier k through the influence of a following front vowel are: O.E. ċin(n), Mod.E. ‘chin,’ with which compare Gothic kinnus, O.E. cyċene, an early loan-word from Latin coquīna, through an intermediate form, *kukina. In this O.E. word the second k was fronted before the front vowel i, whereas the initial remains a back consonant, because the following y, although also a front vowel, did not become so until the tendency for such vowels to affect preceding consonants had passed away. These processes will be described later on in more detail, in dealing specifically with O.E. sound changes.

Another combinative tendency which affects a large number of words in O.E. was that to round back vowels before nasal consonants. Thus we have reason to know that the O.E. mōna, ‘moon,’ came from an earlier form, *mānō, with the unrounded (ā) (mid- or low-back) in the first syllable. It is probable that the vowel itself was first slightly nasalized, and this nasal (ā̃) gradually tended to acquire a rounded pronunciation, just as the nasal vowel in en, an, in French, as in enfant (ãfã), is rounded, in the pronunciation of most French speakers, sometimes to a very considerable extent.

Now, it is characteristic of all tendencies of change in pronunciation, both Isolative and Combinative, that they obtain only for a period in the history of a language, and then pass away. Thus, for instance, as we have seen at a certain time, the speakers of Old English tended to pronounce back consonants before front vowels more and more forward, until at last they were uttered as wholly front consonants. But this habit died out, since we find that this modification of back consonants does not take place before those front vowels which were developed by a DYING OUT OF TENDENCIES OF CHANGE later process from earlier back vowels. We pronounce, to the present day, a back consonant in ‘kin,’ and therefore can have no doubt that the O.E. word cynn, ‘race,’ ‘family,’ also had a back consonant (k) initially, although the next sound in the word, y (high-front-round), is just as much a front vowel as i in O.E. ċin, ‘chin.’ But O.E. y in the former word was originally u, as we can see from a comparison with the Gothic kuni, which preserves the older form of the vowel. The O.E. y sound was developed by a fronting of original u, at a period at which there was no longer any tendency on the part of English speakers to advance the place of articulation of k when it came immediately before a front vowel.

According to the varying speech habits, the same combination of sounds is differently treated, not only in different dialects or languages, but in the same language at different periods. The so-called Sound Laws, or Phonetic Laws, therefore, are merely statements to the effect that at a given time, a given community tended to alter the pronunciation of such and such a sound, or combination of sounds, in such and such a way. This, of course, does not prevent the same tendency arising, independently, in totally unrelated languages, or more than once in the same language.

The problem of combinative changes is no less difficult than that of isolative changes. It is true that, in the former case, the immediate phonetic or physiological causes which determine the change are generally apparent; but these causes are not of universal operation, as we have seen from the fact that different languages, or the same language at different periods of its history, may treat the same combination of sounds in different ways, now leaving it unaltered, now altering it in this way or that.

This transitoriness of tendencies of sound change has already been illustrated by those combinative processes in the history of English to which passing reference has been made, but further illustration may be useful to show with what varying force they obtain, even among the different dialects of the same language.

A good example of this is the process known as ‘u-å-Umlaut,’ which began in O.E., probably early in the eighth century. Briefly stated, this process consisted in the development of a vowel-glide after a front vowel when a back rounded vowel follows in the next syllable. This vowel-glide apparently develops into a full vowel, which combines with the preceding to produce a diphthong. Thus an original widu, ‘wood,’ becomes *wiᵘdu, then wiudu, whence wiodu in Northumbrian, and weodu (wudu) in Mercian and West Saxon.

The O.E. dialects vary considerably, both in the extent to which this diphthonging takes place, and also in the conditions which promote its occurrence.

In West Saxon, Northumbrian, and part of the Kentish area, œ remains unaffected by a following u, o, a; in Mercian, on the other hand, original œ, when followed by one of these vowels, is diphthongized, first to œᵘ, œu, œo, œa, ea, the latter being the ordinary spelling. Thus in W.S. and Northumbrian the plural of fœt, ‘cup,’ ‘vessel’ (Mod.E. ‘vat’), is fatu, from *fœtu, with un-fronting of œ to a before the following u, but in Mercian featu.

The vowels i and e are diphthongized, to a certain extent, in all dialects, but the conditions under which this UNEQUAL DEVELOPMENT occurs are far more limited in W.S. than in the other dialects; also u produces diphthongization much more readily in this dialect than a or o. Thus, after w, i became iu < io < eo quite normally, no matter what the intervening consonant may be: cwicu, ‘living,’ becomes cweocu; widu < weodu (whence, later, c(w)ucu, wudu), otherwise the vowel remains undiphthongized, except when l, r, or the lip consonants intervene: sicol, ‘sickle,’ from *sikul, nigun, ‘nine,’ from *niȝun, sinu, ‘sinew,’ hnitu, ‘nit’; but sweotol (and swutol), ‘clear,’ from *switul, meolc (earlier miuluc), from *miluk, ‘milk,’ seofon, ‘seven,’ from *siƀun, cleopode, ‘called,’ from *cliupode, earlier clipode, pret. of clipian, and so on.

Under approximately the same conditions original e becomes eu, then eo: eofor, ‘wild boar,’ from eƀur, heorot, ‘hart,’ from earlier herut, heolstor, ‘darkness,’ from earlier helustor; but regol, ‘rule,’ an early loan-word from the Latin regula, fetor, ‘fetter,’ from *fetur, sprecol, from earlier sprecul, ‘loquacious.’

It appears, from the above examples, that in W.S. the tendency to diphthongization did not arise when the intervening consonant was a point-teeth or back, unless w preceded the i or e.

In the Kentish dialect of O.E., on the other hand, i and e, and, in some early texts, œ also, appear to be diphthongized, whenever u follows in the next syllable, whether w precedes or not, and no matter what the nature of the intervening consonant. Thus we find such forms as reogol, ‘rule,’ breogo, ‘prince,’ from *bregu, freođu- (in names), when W.S. has friđu-. Such Kentish forms as ‘to niomanne,’ ‘to take,’ forgeofan (inf.), earlier *-geƀan, where i and e are diphthongized by a following a, are quite foreign to W.S., which has nimanne, giefan (also from *geƀan, by a process peculiar to W.S. p. 236).

Mercian and Northumbrian also diphthongize i and e freely; the former œ as well, but before a following back consonant (c or g) the diphthong is ‘smoothed’ or monophthongized again, in these dialects, by a tendency which arose subsequent to the u-, a-, o-Umlaut. Thus in Mercian *dœgum, dœgas (dat. and nom.-acc. pl. of dœg, ‘day’) apparently became *dœᵘgum, etc., but were subsequently smoothed to dœgum, dœgas, which are the forms actually found in the principal Mercian text (Vespasian Psalter).

These processes of diphthongization did not arise, so far as we know, in any of the O.E. dialects before the beginning of the eighth or, at earliest, the end of the seventh century, and when once the above changes were complete, the speech habit which produced them died out, never again to be revived.[C]


A very full account, and copious illustrations of every class of Isolative and Combinative Sound Change, will be found in Paul Passy’s Changements Phonétiques du Langage, Paris, 1891.

It might appear that the problem of Combinative Change differs essentially from that of Isolative Change, since in the former case the ‘causes’ can be discovered and stated, whereas in the latter case it is only possible to state that this or that change occurs, undetermined, however, so far as we can discover, by the nature of the surrounding sounds. But since, as we have seen, the ‘causes’ of Combinative Change depend for their effectiveness upon the natural speech tendencies which obtain at the moment throughout a community, it is evident that CAUSES OF SOUND CHANGE the real determining ‘cause’ of this class of sound changes, as of isolative changes, is the speech basis. It is the general habit of speech which produces among a group of speakers the tendency to a given treatment of a combination of sounds, no less than to that of the isolated sound. Some German writers (e.g., Sievers, in his Phonetik) employ the terms ‘bedingt,’ or ‘caused,’ sound change for combinative, as distinct from ‘unbedingt,’ or ‘uncaused,’ for isolative change. These terms are misleading, unless it be clearly borne in mind that both classes of change are ultimately caused or determined by the natural tendencies which are inseparable from a given speech basis. It is only by virtue of this that the pronunciation of a sound, at a given moment in the history of a language, tends to be influenced by the surrounding sounds.

We cannot explain the reason of the rise and passing away of these tendencies; we can only shift the matter a stage further back, and say that they are inseparably associated with the speech basis of the community at the moment, and that, since this is unstable, so also the tendencies to variation must necessarily be in different directions at different times and among different communities.

The real problem of the causes of sound change, then, is put in the question, What factors determine the precise nature of the speech basis of a community at a particular period? If we could answer this question, we should solve the question which is involved in it, namely, Why do the speakers of a community show at one period a set of tendencies in pronunciation, a group of speech habits, which are quite foreign to their ancestors or their descendants in former or later ages?—we should be far nearer than we are at present to solving one of the most important problems connected with the evolution of speech.

Many attempts have been made to account for the general fact that the sounds of language change, but none are wholly satisfactory. The simple question, What is it that modifies the speech basis of a community? remains unanswered, or, at best, only partially answered.

Formerly all sound change was ascribed to the inherent laziness of men, who were said to be for ever striving after increased ease of utterance. This was the view of the eminent philologist Schleicher (Deutsche Sprache, pp. 50 and following) and Whitney the Sanscrit scholar (Language and its Study, 1875, pp. 42, 43, and Life and Growth of Language, 1886, p. 49, etc.). It must be urged against this theory that ease and difficulty are very relative terms—familiar sounds being, as a rule, easy, unfamiliar sounds difficult; and although a certain absolute difficulty might, perhaps, be asserted to exist in certain sound combinations, they are nevertheless preserved in some languages. Some changes which occur in language seem to be in the direction rather of increased than less effort. The real answer, however, is that the fact of ease or difficulty existing among a given community in the pronunciation of certain sounds depends upon their speech basis.

A desire for Euphony is another popular explanation, which formerly received the support of authorities—e.g., Bopp, Vgl. Gr., pp. 7, 77, 96, 274, etc.; Vocalismus, pp. 18, 29; also Scherer, Geschichte d. deutschen Spr., pp. 136-138. This suggestion must be at once rejected when we reflect that pronunciation changes gradually, without the deliberate intention, or even the knowledge, of INFLUENCE OF CLIMATE the speakers; and, further, that the deliberate alteration of pronunciation for the purpose of producing a more beautiful effect upon the ear would make sound change largely a matter of personal whim, which would result in endless diversity—to the extent of imperilling intelligibility—within the same community.

The influence of Climate was pressed by Osthoff (Das physiologische und das psychologische Moment in der Sprachlichen Formenbildung, 1879) as a means of accounting for the diversity of treatment of the same original sounds among the various groups of Aryan speakers. It cannot be denied that climate, since it determines so largely the general mode of life, the social organization, and the bodily habits of a community, and originally possibly even the racial characters must also, to some extent, at least, affect the language. And yet the sounds of a language go on changing throughout the centuries, while the people continue to live under the same climatic conditions. It would seem more probable that climate might help to predispose the speech basis of a community in a new direction, if a tribe migrated from its original seat to a new and very different geographical area, but that when the climatic conditions had once produced their effect, or continued to produce them upon each succeeding generation, they would rather tend to conserve than to alter the speech basis, unless, of course, some marked change of climate came about. At any rate, so far, no specific sound change has ever been related, with certainty, to any definite conditions of climate, and it seems as if the most that we can say is, that climate may contribute to produce a speech basis which inherently tends to vary along certain lines, although the connection between the two has never yet been shown.

Darmsteter (La Vie des Mots, 1887, p. 7) and Passy (Changements Phonétiques du Langage, 1891, pp. 230-235) maintain that sound change is primarily due to the ‘mistakes’ and faulty imitation of the pronunciation of their elders by children when learning to speak. This amounts to saying that children never perfectly master the sounds of their native language, a view which seems to be contradicted by experience; for the grosser ‘mistakes’ of children are soon corrected, and at seven or eight years of age the normal child is usually completely conversant with all the sounds in use among the community in which he lives. Besides, it is not explained how it comes about that all the children of the same generation make approximately the same ‘mistakes’; or, in other words, why, if sound change has its roots in ‘mistakes’ of this kind, the pronunciation of a given community tends to vary on practically homogeneous lines. It is, of course, true that language changes from generation to generation, in the very process, as we have seen, of being handed on, but this is because the rising generation begins, as it were, where the former leaves off; their speech is the reproduction of the most recent developments of their parents’ speech, and has, therefore, a slightly different starting-point of deviation. Thus, if the norm of the parents’ speech be represented by a, with a possible, unperceived deviation represented by a4, the children’s norm will perhaps be a³, with the range of possibilities of deviation, bringing the limit to a7. There is also an element of variation in the fact that individuals are differently constituted, mentally FOREIGN CONTACT and physically, so that the learner’s speech can never be an exact reproduction of that of his parents. But these personal peculiarities in speech cannot, normally, exceed the limits at which they are recognisable.

Lastly, in enumerating the various explanations proposed, we may mention the factor which has been emphasized by Hirt (Indogermanische Forschungen, iv., pp. 36-45), and quite recently, and more fully, by Wechssler (Gibt es Lautgesetze? 1900), as chief among the influences which modify the speech basis—namely, contact with foreign speakers.

The nature of this influence is easily grasped. In attempting to reproduce the sounds of a foreign language we inevitably, as has been already pointed out, attempt to imitate the strange sounds by uttering those sounds which are nearest to them, according to our own perceptions, in our own language. We never completely acquire the new series of movements—that is, the speech basis of the foreign tongue—but tend to modify the sounds, according to our own familiar habits of articulation. Thus in time may we indeed acquire a new speech basis, one different from our own, but differing, also, more or less, from that of the language we are trying to speak. The result is practically a new form of speech which is neither one thing nor the other. If we conceive of this process on a much larger scale, as when two races come into social contact and acquire each other’s language, subsequently the speech of one will predominate, that of the other dying out, with the result that the speech basis of the whole area occupied by the two groups of speakers has been shifted: first in the mouths of the foreigners, and then, if these and their descendants are really assimilated, so that the two races are welded into a single community, by the reaction of the new manner of speech on the old. In the primitive wanderings of races the process of the incorporation of peoples speaking different languages must continually be going on.

The further question of how far racial characteristics tell in moulding the speech basis, is also involved in the above hypothesis. Are we to add race mixture as a further influence on the language arising from foreign contact?

It seems evident that such obvious points as the degree of thickness of the lips, the length and general size of the tongue, the facial angle, the shape and size of the nose, all of which are characteristic racial features, must play a considerable part in determining the original speech basis; and there may be subtler points of anatomical structure which play a part, as well as the general temperament and natural bodily habit.

But so far the anatomists have done but little to show the precise connection between the physical structure of races and the speech basis therewith associated.

In the absence of precise knowledge it is, perhaps, safer to assume that, within limits, the speech organs are so adaptable that an individual of any race can acquire the speech habits of any other, provided his linguistic training begins in childhood, and that the structural differences between the vocal organs of the various races are of less importance, on the whole, in determining the speech basis, than are those particular habits of using the organs, which are acquired in infancy by the unconscious and natural process of learning the mother-tongue, understanding by RACIAL CHARACTERISTICS this phrase the language which a child learns first.

It seems that a change in the speech basis need not imply a modification in the structure of the speech organs themselves, but only of the mode of using them.

At the same time, it is a reasonable inference that the speech basis is, under normal conditions, related to the actual shape and structure of the organs of speech, and therefore that the more two races differ in physical type, the greater will be the differences in their natural speech habits. In this sense, the effect of foreign speakers in modifying the speech basis of a community, will be in proportion to the degree of separation between the two races. The more unlike one race is to another in temperament and physical type, the greater will be the difference between the natural tendencies of their speech organs; the more considerable, therefore, the modification which the language of each will undergo in the mouths of speakers of the other race.

The views of Hirt and Wechssler are widely accepted at the present moment, and there can be no doubt that the suggestion which they contain is a most valuable one in explaining, for instance, the differences which exist between the several groups of the Aryan family of languages, or the different branches of the Latin tongues—Italian, Spanish, French, Provençal, etc., all of which have been developed from closely-allied forms of popular Latin; but the explanation does not always apply to the case where a single language in the course of its history develops, as we have seen is the case in English, quite different tendencies in succeeding periods, without it being possible to show the connection between these tendencies, and any specific characteristic in other languages which have come into contact with it by conquest or otherwise. It might be maintained that those well-marked sound changes which distinguish Old English from the other West Germanic languages are, in some obscure way, due to the influence of native British speakers of Celtic origin, and later on of Scandinavians, and that the impulse to the sound changes which characterize the Middle English period had its origin in the speech of the Normans; but even if such a theory could be substantiated, which is in the highest degree improbable, what foreign influence is responsible for the very considerable changes which have taken place in English pronunciation since the sixteenth century?

A factor which has hitherto hardly been considered, and which has certainly not been systematically investigated, is Occupation. There can be little doubt that the prolonged use of certain parts of the body in a particular way tends not only to affect the form and function of the parts themselves, but also, indirectly, induces a certain general bodily habit. There are many such modifications of the individual which affect the organs of speech, and may predispose the person concerned to a particular mode of using these. Thus it might be supposed that such work as swinging a scythe or flail would develop the muscles of the chest and throat, in such a way as to affect the utterance. Again, the constant necessity to shout, which exists in noisy occupations, such as that of the fisherman or sailor, who has to make himself heard through the storm, or that of the blacksmith or factory hand, who must make their voices rise above the clang of the hammer on the anvil, or the hum and clashing of machinery, can but produce a permanent INFLUENCE OF OCCUPATION habit of speaking loud, which may affect the quality of the sounds uttered. Another point is that in speaking from a distance or amid noise, certain speech sounds become practically useless, because they are inaudible—namely, voiceless consonants, especially the stops. Under these conditions the vowels are all-important, particularly those of the stressed syllables. These remarks are merely thrown out as a suggestion of a possible source of the modification of the speech basis. In any case, occupation can hardly be omitted from the forces which affect the development of language.

Of all the above factors which, it has been maintained, modify the speech basis, none can be considered wholly sufficient to explain all cases; and, although we may admit that race, climate, occupation, and foreign contact, each and all play their part in determining the physical and mental habits of a community, we must also recognise that the whole question is still very obscure, and that at present we know neither the precise way in which speech is affected by these modifying factors, nor how any of them, while remaining to all appearance constant, can yet produce tendencies of change, now in this way, now in that, in the pronunciation of a single language.

In fact, so far as the history of a single language is concerned, which is spoken for a long period by the same race, in the same geographical area, and under identical climatic conditions, unaffected, for long periods at any rate, by any alien language, it is hardly too much to say that, although we can understand why the pronunciation should indeed be liable to change, we can, as yet, form no idea as to why such a language develops just those specific changes in its sound system which, as a matter of fact, actually occur, nor why these arise at one period rather than another. For the present, the words of M. Paul Passy (Changements Phonétiques, § 617) remain true: ‘En somme, ce que nous savons sur les causes premières des changements phonétiques est bien peu de chose. Nous constatons que dans tel dialecte, à tel moment, telle ou telle tendance phonétique prédomine; pourquoi prédomine-t-elle, nous l’ignorons, ou nous pouvons tout au plus le conjecturer.’


CHAPTER V

DIFFERENTIATION OF LANGUAGE: THE RISE OF DIALECTS

The problem now before us is how, from an originally uniform and homogeneous form of speech, there are developed, in the course of time, innumerable varieties—dialects which differ in varying degrees one from the other in essential features of pronunciation, and languages which are so distinct that only the most searching historical investigation can reveal their original affinity.

We may say at once that there is no radical difference between a ‘Dialect’ and a ‘Language.’ From the moment that two forms of speech present what we somewhat vaguely call ‘dialectal’ differences, which mark them as separate, the potentialities exist for infinite divergence. Under favourable conditions the two dialects may grow wider and wider apart, until not only are the two groups of speakers mutually unintelligible, but their common origin could never be suspected without the application of rigid historical and comparative method.

The distinction between a ‘Dialect’ and a ‘Language’ is only one of the degree of differentiation from original type.

We have seen that the starting-point of sound change lies in the individual speaker. A change in the speech of a community is the result of the tendencies of a host of individuals. It has been pointed out that every individual differs slightly from every other; how, then, can we speak of a community possessing a homogeneous language? Further, we may ask, What is the precise relation of the speech of the individual to that of the community?

It is as well to know clearly what we mean by the term ‘community,’ and it may be defined, for purposes of linguistic discussion, as a group of individuals who, by reason chiefly of the frequency of their social intercourse, naturally use the same form of speech, and among whom the individual differences are so slight that they are inappreciable. We speak of the ‘community at large,’ generally meaning thereby all persons who live in these islands. But within this large group of human beings there are many smaller groups and sections of the community. The smaller the social division, the closer must be the bond between the members of it, the more frequent and intimate their intercourse. Thus the inhabitants of a province, county, or large city form a little community or State by themselves, whose members are to a great extent independent of, and shut off from the influence of, other counties and cities. Normally, the communication and opportunities for social intercourse of such a group of persons among themselves are greater than those between them and the members of other similar groups outside their own. But even within the limits of the county or province, still smaller and more closely knit communities exist, in the villages and the hamlets included within the wider division. The hamlets and villages, again, are made up of groups of separate families, and these, the SPEECH COMMUNITIES narrowest and closest of all divisions of society, consist of individuals.

In the strict sense, the limits of a speech community are comparatively narrow. Only such persons who, by virtue of their place of abode, and their occupations, and their general conditions of life, are brought into constant, and more or less intimate social intercourse, can be said to constitute a speech community. In the country, the village is generally coextensive with the speech community; in large towns the population forms itself into speech communities in the narrow sense, on principles which are largely determined by class and occupation; but also to some extent by the actual distribution of the inhabitants throughout the various quarters and districts of the city.

Among the members of the community, in the narrowest sense, there exist not only actual differences of pronunciation, but also differences of tendency—one individual tends to vary his pronunciation in this way, another in that. But these differences of actual pronunciation, and of tendency to change, are usually so slight, that they are unperceived, both by the individual himself and by the community among whom he lives. They arise, as we have seen, quite naturally, from the differences of mental and physical organization; but they do not progress beyond a certain point, partly because of the unconscious effort of the speaker to reproduce exactly the sounds which he habitually hears, and partly because social intercourse, whereby the speech is acquired and handed on, no less than the fact that all the speakers of the community are under practically identical conditions of life, naturally contributes to produce approximately the same habits of mind and body, therefore the same speech basis, and consequently the same pronunciation, and the same tendencies of change, in all the members of the community.

The majority of tendencies of variation in speech habit which exist in the individual will be shared also by the speech community at large, so that they will be strengthened and encouraged by social intercourse. Those tendencies, on the other hand, which are peculiar to the individual, and which are not shared by the community, will not gain ground, but will be eliminated. The strongest and most clearly marked of these individual tendencies will be unconsciously suppressed, or, in some cases even, will be deliberately checked in youth, by the corrective ridicule of associates; others, which are not sufficiently marked to be generally noticeable, either disappear naturally with the definite acquirement of the speech basis, or may continue to exist, so long as they do not develop beyond the point at which they are recognisable by the speaker himself and by his companions. Thus there is in every community a certain body of tendency which is common to all speakers, and this develops, unperceived and gradual, but also, for the time being, unchecked.

Allowing, then, for the slight and unrecognised differences which exist between individual and individual, we may say that the speech of a community, in the special sense above defined, is homogeneous for all practical purposes; and, allowing for the elimination of the purely individual tendencies, which do not jump with the general trend of speech habit, we may further say that all the members of INTERRUPTION OF SOCIAL INTERCOURSE such a community will tend, at a given time, to change their speech basis, and therefore their pronunciation, in one and the same direction.

Now, it is clear that this uniformity of pronunciation, and this agreement in direction of change, presuppose the existence of a community in the sense in which we have defined it—namely, under such conditions that all the members have equal opportunities of intercourse with each other. If, however, this state of things be altered or upset, if circumstances arise which make this social intercourse less frequent, and less intense at any point within the community, or which create conditions in the mode of life which affect the community unequally; then we can no longer regard the groups of speakers thus unequally affected, and variously circumstanced, as one community in the terms of our definition, but must consider that there are as many communities as there are centres of disturbance of the original conditions. We may regard the groups of speakers thus formed as isolated the one from the other, the degree of isolation being measured by the degree of interruption of the social intercourse which formerly existed.

Now, when isolation occurs, which splits one community into two or more groups, the necessary conditions are present for the differentiation of the originally homogeneous speech into dialects. Each group will tend to develop its language along different lines, and the differences, slight enough in the beginning, may in time attain considerable proportions. The reason why the different groups of speakers necessarily grow further and further apart as regards their language is not difficult to understand. We must consider that every individual naturally tends gradually to diverge from the norm in speech so far as is possible within the limits already described. But the question of which of his personal tendencies are allowed to develop, and which are eliminated, is determined by the general balance of habit and tendency in the community as a whole. So soon as the constitution of the community is changed, the balance is upset, and tendencies which would before have been checked may now, among a smaller group of speakers find a wider echo:—that is, there is a larger proportion of speakers who share them. These tendencies, therefore, are confirmed, and may become general among the new and smaller community. Again, tendencies which find encouragement, and gain a firm footing in one community, are eliminated in another. Of course, unless the isolation be complete, it is probable that all the groups of speakers will still have certain lines of change in common, and will also agree, as before, in suppressing, for the most part unconsciously, certain other tendencies.

The formation of dialects depends, then, upon the development of different groups or series of tendencies among communities which are isolated one from the other. The extent to which two or more dialects differ from, or agree with each other, in fostering, or eliminating, this or that tendency to variation, will depend upon the degree of completeness of the isolation of the several communities.

We may now properly inquire what are the chief factors of isolation, or modes of interruption, of social intercourse which split up a community and give rise to dialectal DIVISIONS OF SOCIETY—MODES OF ISOLATION differences.

We may divide human society into groups of increasing size: the Family, a group of individuals naturally associated together by the fact of common parents and a common dwelling-place; the Hamlet or Village, or group of Families; the Province, which includes numerous villages; and the Nation at large, which embraces all—Provinces, Villages, Hamlets, Families, and Individuals.

Each of these divisions, while it typifies characteristic modes of isolation of group from group, necessarily involves also a characteristic association of the members of each group. Individual is isolated from individual, even in the same family, as we have seen, by slight differences of mind and body. These are the psychological and physiological, or Organic factors of isolation. Among them we may also consider differences of Age and of Sex Family is separated from Family by the barriers of Occupation, Class, and the fact of living in different houses—these we may call the Social factors; Hamlet or Village from other Hamlets and Villages by the geographical features of the country—varying distance, rivers, mountain ranges, forests, moors, or lakes, and by what we may call Political conditions. These are the geographical factors, which, of course, include also the Political, Social, and Organic factors. Province is isolated from Province, and Nation from Nation, by the same kind of factors, only they are naturally intensified as the geographical separation becomes greater, until this often involves the further factors of Climate, Soil, the general mode of life, Religion, and Race itself.

The wider our Social divisions, the more powerful, important, and complete becomes the mode of isolation which is associated with it. A community may gradually spread, by a process of natural and steady increase in numbers, over an immense area, until the outlying fringes of population attain to so great a geographical severance from the original centre that they reach an altogether different soil and climate. These may involve a total change in mode of life and in the whole fabric of Society, and contact with new and very different races. On the other hand, instead of the gradual spread of the population over wide tracts of country, the same results may be more rapidly, but just as completely, attained by a section of the community moving off from their original seats, and proceeding, within a comparatively short space of time, to a remote geographical area.

It will be readily recognised that the Geographical factors are the most powerful of all in the differentiation of speech, since not only do they involve the complete isolation which results from a total severance of all social intercourse, thus including, in a very thorough form, all that group of factors which we have called the Social group, but they also expose the speakers to new conditions of Soil and Climate, and all that follows therefrom, and in this way are active in modifying the physical and mental organization, and therefore the speech basis. As we have repeatedly insisted, the speech basis of a people, even when they are living under the same conditions for a long space of time, tends to vary; but this process is greatly hastened and intensified if the community be subjected to such changed conditions of life and such different outward surroundings as those to which it is SOCIAL ISOLATION—CLASS DIALECTS exposed by migration to other climes, far-distant lands, and among alien peoples. We can observe how great are the differences in speech in a single large town between the different classes—the Public Services, the Professions, Commerce in its various grades, the Artisans, the Slum-dwellers. The isolation between these groups is Social, partly the natural result of difference of occupation, partly, also, due to the more artificial barriers of Class or Caste which are closely associated therewith. Originally, probably, the same, the divisions created by Occupation and by Class are now distinct in nature, although they cross each other and overlap at innumerable points.

But with all its differences of dialect, the speech of one large town, taken as a whole, may appear almost homogeneous, if we compare it with that of another town in the same country which is a few hundred miles away. Such towns as Glasgow, Liverpool, and Bristol, all possess a number of what we may call class and occupational dialects, but the differences between such dialects are comparatively slight, by the side of those differences which will appear from a comparison of the speech as a whole, in each of the cities mentioned, with that of the others; that is to say, that those speakers from Glasgow who differ most widely amongst each other, will have far more in common in their several pronunciations, than they will have with any speakers from Liverpool or Bristol. This statement does not, of course, include speakers of Standard English in these cities, whose speech is not appreciably modified by the Regional Dialect.

The social conditions at the present time are so complex that, apart from the inhabitants of small country villages, practically no individual can be regarded merely as the member of a single community. From his position in society, the nature of his avocations, and the place of his abode, almost every one belongs, from these different points of view, to several communities; he is brought, with varying degrees of intimacy, into relations with people of every class, engaged upon all manner of employments, and coming from widely different parts of the country. The result is that the speech of almost every individual, unless, indeed, as we have said, he lives continuously in one small country village, where the social circle is extremely limited, and where communication with the outer world is inconsiderable and infrequent—the speech of every individual does not represent a uniform dialect, as spoken by any single class or community, but is, in reality, a compromise between the characteristics of several different dialects. Consider the case of a wealthy merchant or banker. He spends part of his time in the city, where he associates with persons employed in business similar to his own, some of them his equals in education and social status, others belonging to a different social class, and therefore, often, to a very different speech community. Our banker or merchant has been at a Public School, and at a University; he has spent, perhaps, some years in foreign travel as part of his general training; his wealth enables him to reside in London for part of the year, and also to live in baronial fashion in the country for the other part. Outside his hours of business he associates with his fellow merchant princes, but also with men of the liberal professions, with diplomats, members of Parliament, military men, country gentlemen, peasants, and peers. It is impossible to classify SOCIAL AND LINGUISTIC CONDITIONS such a man merely as either a city merchant, a man about town, a University man, or a country gentleman. He is each and all of these in turn; he belongs to several communities at once, and his speech inevitably bears traces of his contact with, and sojourn among, every one of them, though one or other will preponderate in determining his mode of utterance. It is probable that in the case of our hypothetical merchant prince, the speech of the more distinguished classes, among whom he moves as an equal, will to all intents and purposes be his, especially if he has been familiar with it from childhood; but he will not entirely escape the influences of the other class, occupational, or regional dialects with which he is brought into contact. In fact, every speaker of the ‘standard’ English dialect is subjected to the same complex linguistic influences, and his speech necessarily bears traces, however slight these may be, of other forms of English, whether they be the dialect of a class, of a province, or a blending of both. In the same way, no provincial dialect is completely uninfluenced by standard English on the one hand, and by neighbouring local forms of speech on the other.

It is a remarkable thing how comparatively homogeneous the standard English dialect actually is, and how this form of our language may be heard, with a uniformity of pronunciation and intonation in which minor differences appear to be merged, in the mouths of the educated upper classes in all parts of the country.

This degree of uniformity is due to the free intermixture of all people of a certain amount of wealth, which is rendered possible by the facilities of modern locomotion.

This process of unification is begun at those great meeting-places for the wealthy youth of England—the Public Schools and the older Universities.

This linguistic influence is further carried to all classes of the population, in every nook and corner of England, by the clergy, and to a lesser extent by the national schoolmaster.

The fact is that never, under any social conditions, whether these be the most simple and primitive, or the most complex imaginable, is the isolation of any group of speakers from outside influences absolutely complete. The members of a small linguistic group or community may—indeed, do—enjoy a far greater frequency of intercourse among themselves than do any of them with the members of communities outside. In a primitive state of society it is difficult to draw a distinction between the Homestead, which includes the members of one family and their dependents, and the Hamlet. But the influence of external communities, too, must of necessity be exerted to some extent—directly in some cases, in others indirectly. Thus, no dialect can possibly possess absolute uniformity, for the external influences do not affect all the members equally. New and ‘foreign’ tendencies are acquired by some members and not by others.

A group of families who reside in proximity, in the same hamlet, (or even the divisions of one and the same family) may represent so many separate communities. The isolation of one such family or division from another may not be great, but it is sufficient to allow of each being subject to slightly different external speech influences, or reacting in a slightly different way to the same influence. One family may acquire this peculiarity from the speakers of another village, while another family takes on quite RELATIVE UNIFORMITY OF SPEECH a different habit or tendency. If we took as a test the possession, or the reverse, of these particular habits of speech, it would be necessary to classify the two families as forming two slightly distinct communities, speaking two slightly different dialects. On the other hand, the points in which there was linguistic agreement between the families of the same village would be far in advance, in number and degree, of those in which they differed; so that, bearing in mind the actual facts, we should be justified in asserting that the dialect of the village or homestead was uniform, in the relative sense that the members of that particular village community showed a greater linguistic affinity with each other, than with any other group or groups of speakers.

It is in this qualified and relative sense, that we speak of the uniformity and homogeneity of Primitive Aryan or Primitive Germanic speech. We cannot conceive of any considerable collection of human beings whose speech should not present at least that degree of dialectal differentiation, which must exist between the different families or households that make up the community as a whole. The two principles—individual variation and collective unity—are for ever contrasted in language. As Paul has said (Principien, p. 55), it belongs to the nature of language, as a medium of social intercourse, that the individual speaker should feel himself to be in agreement with his fellows.

Divergencies which originally arise in a single family may, in time, spread to one or more other families, and thence to the whole tribe. If a group of closely allied families move off from the rest of the tribe, and migrate to a distant area, the slight peculiarities which in their original seats differentiated their speech from that of their fellow-tribesmen may form the starting-point for divergencies of considerable magnitude.

It is possible that the beginnings of the dissimilar tendencies among the various Aryan languages in the treatment of lip-modified back consonants, and of the ‘palatalized’ or partly-fronted consonants, may have arisen as slight dialectal divergencies within Primitive Aryan itself.

It is important to realize that the gradual dying out of the old local dialects, which is at present going on, and the levelling up and down of speech, throughout our own country, to a type which appears to offer but an insignificant degree of variety, is not a purely natural process. There is no natural tendency in a language which is already differentiated into various dialects, to become uniform; nor do the impulses towards divergence become weaker with the growth of civilization, and the spread of education. The phenomenon which we are witnessing in England to-day, is that of one dialect being gradually substituted for others. That such a substitution should occur is not a new thing in the history of language; it depends in our own case upon the prestige of the encroaching dialect, as well as upon social conditions. The degree of uniformity with which the standard dialect is spoken over a large area, depends upon the extent to which the factors of geographical and social isolation can be weakened. At the present day, this is undoubtedly effected to a certain extent, partly by the mixture of classes, which characterizes our social system, partly, also, by the great development in means of communication between different THE DREAM OF A UNIVERSAL LANGUAGE parts of the country, which has taken place during the last fifty years, chief among which we must, of course, place railway extension; but we must by no means disregard the influence of the bicycle and the motor-car.

Still, it is easy to over-estimate the degree of uniformity which exists in English speech, and a minute investigation by a trained observer, will reveal differences which are very real, but which easily escape the notice of the untrained ear.

The need of a uniform international language has of late years been forcibly urged, and to-day there are probably many thousands of persons all over Europe who can speak Esperanto. It is interesting to speculate as to the probable future of this movement. From what we know concerning the changes of languages, it seems probable that if this artificial language were really to become firmly established in all the civilized countries of the world, it could not long retain a sufficient degree of uniformity, either in structure, or in pronunciation, to serve the purpose for which it was originally created. At the present moment, there is a conventional pronunciation which can be approximately acquired, with fair ease, by the natives of most countries. But, already, every speaker must necessarily modify the sounds in a certain way, in accordance with the speech basis of his mother-tongue. Thus an Englishman will diphthongize (ō) and (ē) to (ou) and (ɛi); a Russian will make ō into (ɔ̄)—that is, low-back-tense-round; a Swede will either over-round this sound, (ō), till the effect produced upon foreign ears is that of (ū), or will attempt to reproduce it by (ɔ). Again, such a sound as (ū), = high-back-tense-round, will be made by the Swede into the high-flat-tense-round or the mid-back-tense-over-rounded, and by the Frenchman into a high-back-tense-round with considerable advancing of the tongue; a Welshman will make (ō) and (ē) into (ɔ̄) and (ɛ̄), and so on. This for a beginning. But when once the language has been learnt, and has become a traditional form of speech, as is presumably hoped by those who advocate its use, its sounds will develop on different lines in every country, since, as they will be identical with the corresponding sounds in the native language, they will, of course, follow precisely the same path of change as that which these pursue. Thus we should expect that in a few generations Esperanto will be different in each country, so far as the sounds are concerned. Added to the difficulty of diffusing a uniform sound system among widely-separated peoples, each speaking a distinct language of their own, we must further consider the equally formidable difficulty of preserving a uniform system of accent, including thereunder both stress and intonation. Frenchmen will never, as a nation, acquire a system of strong stress on certain syllables of words, with weak stresses on the others, such as exists in Italian or the Germanic languages. A very slight error in the distribution of stress is sufficient to make a word unintelligible. The present writer has repeatedly heard a Frenchman pronounce the word ‘literature’ (litératjūr) instead of (lítərətʃə) or (lítrətʃə), with the result that a group of Englishmen who were present, were completely baffled as to what he meant. The same Frenchman also spoke of the works of (bɛrnártʃau), whom the writer took to be a Chinese author, until it appeared from the conversation that Mr. Bernard Shaw (bʌ̄́nədʃɔ́) was referred to.

It is difficult, at present, to see how divergencies of this POSSIBILITIES OF ESPERANTO kind can be avoided, in the pronunciation of Esperanto; and if they exist, not only will the new language lack uniformity from the beginning, but the subsequent divergencies in the different countries will be all the greater from the fact that the starting-points will be diverse to begin with, and the tendencies which mould the future destinies of the various forms will be different in each case. It may be argued that the facilities of international communication are rapidly developing, that the geographical isolation between even the mutually remotest countries of the world will, in time, be no more insuperable than that between the North and South of England at the present day, or again, that the increased use of telephonic communication may make it as easy to converse with a man in St. Petersburg as with one in the same room. We must admit that progress in the utilization of steam, electricity, and mechanical contrivances generally, has done much, and will doubtless do yet more, to break down the isolation imposed by distance; but this can never wholly disappear—nothing can ever make social intercourse between persons who habitually live hundreds of thousands of miles from each other, as easy, intimate, and frequent as that between individuals living in the same village, or between communities separated only by a few miles of road or rail. Thus, while the differentiation of language may become increasingly slow, the process must always continue.

The general structure, the word-order, and form of the sentence in such an artificial language as Esperanto must of necessity be profoundly affected in the different centres in which it is cultivated, by the native idiom, since there are no models, as in the case of Latin, to serve as guides. Latin is no longer susceptible of development, so long as the classical models are followed; it is crystallized once for all, and any departure from the old usage is jealously avoided. Nevertheless, in the Medieval Latinity the language is so far a living and traditional instrument of expression, that it was variously affected by the native dialects of the different countries where it was written, so far as structure and idiom are concerned. Immutability in speech is inconceivable, so long as it remains a living expression of thought and emotion, which has its roots in the national consciousness. A language can only cease to change, when it has ceased to live. Change is the necessary penalty which is paid for life, by any form of speech. If Esperanto, so it would appear, ever becomes a living language, it will change, and change in different ways among different groups of human beings. In this case it will no longer serve as a means of international communication. In fact, this purpose can only be realized if Esperanto never actually quickens, but always remains a mere artificial and lifeless collection of words, pronounced according to carefully-drawn rules (which must be learnt afresh by each speaker, and rigidly adhered to), and built up into sentences according to rules upon which all the Esperantists must agree. In this case, doubtless, it will be possible for students from all parts of the world to hold with each other a kind of restricted intercourse both by word of mouth and in writing. The interesting and curious point will be, that from time to time, the natural developments, which are bound to creep in with extensive usage, will need to be deliberately suppressed by congress after congress, as the heresies of the early Church CONDITIONS OF DIFFERENTIATION—SUMMARY were by the Councils.

Such is what might be expected, from what we know of the differentiation of language, to happen to Esperanto, as to any other living form of speech, which has a wide geographical diffusion.


In the last chapter we dealt with the way in which the language of an individual changes, and also discussed briefly the various determining causes of sound change which various writers have suggested. The present chapter has been an attempt to show how, when factors come into play which bring a group of individuals into close social relationship with each other, and at the same time cut them off from other groups of speakers, sound change, which is natural and inevitable, in the speech of all groups, yet takes place in each group along lines more or less different. It has been said that the origin of this differentiation, was the fact that in each group of speakers a different set of tendencies gets the upper hand, while each group also, unconsciously, eliminates on different principles. The various interplay of individual tendencies produces, in each community, a net result which is special and characteristic.

The relative agreement and homogeneity in the speech of the members of the same community was attributed to the unconscious subordination and elimination of idiosyncrasies, and the approximation by the individual of his speech to that of the average of the community. It has been further repeatedly pointed out that the line of development followed by the pronunciation of a community, is determined by the particular line of gradual shifting of the speech basis, and this in its turn is the result of a combination of those general factors already referred to. A few words may be in place here as to the part which these factors play in the speech of the community considered as an association of individuals. It is well to observe that a given set of factors—the Climatic or the Occupational—may, and often do, affect, directly, and equally, all the individuals of a community; but it must not be forgotten that this is not necessarily the case. In the case where the modifying influences of occupation, for instance, act directly, and to the same degree, upon a whole group of individuals it is natural to expect that the results, allowing, of course, for the differences of individual temperament and organization, so often insisted upon, will be the same for all—that is, that the whole group will undergo the same kind of modification of the speech basis.

On the other hand, it must be remembered that the modifying factors may operate by affecting only a few individuals of a group directly, and that the results of this direct influence upon their speech may, through social intercourse, gradually spread to all the other members, although the majority of them have never been directly exposed to that particular source of modification which induces the change in the speech basis. Thus, in the speech of the individual, it is possible, theoretically, to distinguish on the one hand, those alterations of his speech basis which are the result of the direct modification of his habits of speech, or of the actual organs themselves, by external factors, such as occupation, climate, etc.; and on the other those which he acquires by the unconscious imitation of other speakers. A single individual might, under favourable conditions, be the originator of far-reaching modifications in the speech basis of a large community. For this to come about it would be necessary that the peculiarity gained ground, in the first instance, in a very restricted community, such as a family in which the individual, perhaps as father or chief, had considerable influence. Thence the change might easily affect an ever-widening circle. The smaller the social circle involved, and the more limited its relations with larger divisions of society, the less chance there is of the purely individual peculiarities being swamped and eliminated by the speech of the majority. Such considerations bring home to us how complex may be the question of the rise of this or that departure in a language from the former speech habit; since, although, by the time a linguistic phenomenon comes under the observation of science, it may be wide-spread, and appear in a whole family of languages, it may, nevertheless, have had its origin in a remote past, in some obscure and subtle influence exerted upon a very small speech community.

It is probable that in the history of a language different groups of factors co-operate, with varying force, at different periods—now one group predominate in influence, now another. But at present our analysis of causes does not enable us to do more than suggest in a general way, the probable nature of the modifying factors at work; we are for the most part unable to see the precise connection between the effects which we chronicle, and any specific one of the possible causes which may have produced them.

Before concluding this chapter, it may be appropriate to say something of the conception of ‘Laws of Sound Change,’ ‘Phonetic Laws,’ or ‘Sound Laws,’ as they are DIFFERENTIATION OF LANGUAGE variously called, which plays so important a part in modern historical linguistic study.

The phrase is used to express several slightly different ideas, but, reduced to the simplest form, a sound law is merely a statement of the observed facts of pronunciation of a given language at a particular period. The statement that at the present day in the South of England the r-sounds have no trill, but are varieties of a weak point-open consonant, is a sound law. This is the simplest form of sound law. Again, we may state more precisely the phonetic conditions within the word or sentence, under which a sound occurs at a certain period in the history of a language, as when we say that the definite article in English has the vowel (ī) when stressed: ‘he is the one man I want to see’ (hi iz ðī wan mæn ai wɔnt tə sī)—(i) when unstressed, before a word beginning with a vowel; (ə) when unstressed, before a consonant. Both forms are shown in ‘the earth is the Lord’s’ (ði ʌ̄þ iz ðə lɔ̄dz). If we compare the form of a word in more than one period of the same language, we often note that the sound which was pronounced in the earlier has been replaced by another sound in the later period. The statement that O.H.G. (ū) has ‘become,’ or been replaced by, (au) in Mod. H.G.—e.g., O.H.G. mūs, Mod. Ger. maus—is a sound law which is revealed by historical grammar. Lastly, we apply the term ‘sound law’ to the facts of differentiation revealed by the comparison of the forms of the same word in more than one cognate language. The result of comparing Sanscrit šatam, ‘hundred,’ Gk. ἑκατóν, Lat. centum, Gothic hund, Lithuanian szimtas, is that we can formulate the law that a certain original sound, which we will for the THE IMPORTANCE OF PHONOLOGY moment call x, has become š (ʃ) in Scrt., k in Gk. and Lat., h (= χ) in Gmc., sz (= ʃ) in Lithuanian.

This inquiry into the particular series of substitution of sounds, or ‘sound changes,’ which occur in languages at a given moment in their life-history is a very important part of the modern science of language in its historical and comparative aspects. This branch of inquiry, known as Phonological investigation, is at the base of all scientific linguistic study; and the reason for this is obvious when we reflect that unless we know the habits and tendencies to change which characterize a language, or family of languages, we cannot identify, with any degree of certainty, the same word in the various forms it may assume in different ages and in different languages. Until we can take this preliminary step, we cannot profitably compare the forms of one language with the cognate forms in another. We could not know that Irish iasc was cognate with Latin piscis and with English fish, unless we knew from other sources that initial p is lost in Celtic, but becomes f in Gmc.

We have repeatedly insisted in this and the foregoing chapters, that change in language takes place unconsciously—that there is nothing arbitrary or whimsical about it. It has been said that each speaker can diverge to a certain extent from the norm in pronunciation without the divergence being apparent to himself or his fellows. This means that every speaker has a certain group of slight varieties of sound, upon which he rings the changes, all of which, in his consciousness, to his muscular sensations, and to his sense of hearing, represent one and the same sound. Every time he utters a word containing a particular sound, he produces one or other of the varieties which represent his conception of the sound. He may utter now this, now that variety, but he does not go outside the limits imposed by his powers of discrimination of sound and sensation. We may say, therefore, with the above qualification, that the speaker will always pronounce the same sound in the same way. What is true of the individual is true also of the community; and, with qualifications of the kind just made, we may assert that, in a given community, at a given period, the same sound will be pronounced in the same way, whenever it occurs under the same conditions—that is, unless it be affected by the neighbouring sounds in word or sentence.

This is what is meant by the statement, which the school of Leskien, Brugmann, Osthoff, Paul, and Sievers have raised into a cardinal axiom of method, that ‘sound laws admit of no exceptions.’ When apparent exceptions are found it means either—(1) That there are combinative factors at work which we have omitted from our calculation—that is, that the sound is affected by other sounds in the same word, or sentence, or by accent. (2) That the particular word in which the apparent exception occurs, contains a sound which is in reality different in origin, or which has been earlier differentiated from the other sounds with which we had classified it. Cases (1) and (2) necessitate the restatement of our law, or the formulation of a new law, as the case may be. (3) A word may be borrowed from another dialect or language, in which it is pronounced in a different way from the ordinary form in the native dialect. ‘Exceptions’ INADMISSIBILITY OF ‘EXCEPTIONS’ of this order are found in all dialects, which is what we should expect from what has been said with regard to the influence constantly exerted by one dialect upon another. In standard or literary dialects loan-forms from a variety of dialects are particularly frequent. In fact, most literary forms of speech are, to a great extent, artificial products, and represent rather a mixture of elements from several dialects, than any one uniform dialect. Hence a literary language is a far less favourable field for the observation of the laws of the evolution of speech, than an unwritten peasant dialect. (4) The apparent exception may be a form which has not developed by the ordinary processes of sound change from an older form, but due to the Analogy of another form in the same grammatical category, or with which some mental association has been formed. The question of Analogy will be dealt with subsequently.

Having regard to the above facts, the mutual influence of dialects upon each other, and the consequent absence of absolute uniformity of speech, except within the narrowest limits of small communities,—while even here there are the ‘dialects’ of the individuals to be reckoned with,—it is clear that any statement that such and such a sound becomes such and such another, at a given period in a given dialect, can only be an approximation to the actual facts. Thus, when we say that the eighteenth-century English vowel (ǣ) became (ā) in the standard English of the next century—e.g., eighteenth-century (pǣst, lǣf, pǣþ) = present-day (pāst, lāf, pāþ)—we select a particular average type from among several varieties of pronunciation. If we were to examine the pronunciation of these words by a hundred Englishmen at the present day, all from more or less the same class, and who had received the same kind of education, we might possibly find a dozen or more slightly different vowels among them, all of which might be roughly classified as varieties of long (ā), while some of the number might possibly retain some form of the eighteenth-century vowel. The individual varieties of the first class would come under our law, while the others would be classed as dialectal variants, due to the influence of provincial forms of speech, in which the law did not obtain—that is, in which the change of (ǣ) to (ā) had not taken place. A full and complete history of a language would involve an account of the speech of every individual.

In the spelling of Middle English many dialectal varieties of pronunciation, and doubtless also of individual peculiarities, are expressed; but in a highly-cultivated literary language the spelling is usually crystallized, and expresses merely a general average of the extant pronunciations, the same symbol being used by ‘correct’ writers without regard to differences. Thus we must be prepared to admit that such symbols as Greek ω, Latin ū, Gothic ai, which, for practical purposes of philological statement and investigation, we consider as representing severally the same sound, (ō, ū, ai) respectively, with perfect consistency, may in reality have been conventionally used, in the same words, by writers whose pronunciation differed more or less considerably. In all cases, however, until a spelling has become absolutely fixed, like that of classical Greek and Latin or Modern English, it is safe to assume that the use of the symbol is fairly consistent, and that it expresses, INDIVIDUAL DIVERSITY at the worst, a group of closely-related varieties of sound.

So much stress has been laid upon the varieties which exist in what is treated for scientific purposes as a unity—namely, that group of individual dialects which we call a single language, or homogeneous dialect—because these differences, although they are not lost sight of by philological scholars when they assert that the laws of sound change admit of no exceptions, and speak of ‘uniform’ languages and dialects, are yet very apt to be totally ignored by less experienced students, to the great detriment of method, and obscuring of ideas. Each individual, we must remember, pronounces the same sound, whenever it occurs, according to the character of his speech basis, and what is true of the individual is true also of the community. The net result of the regularity and consistency of individual habit and tendency, is consistency of general tendency in such a collection of individual dialects as goes to make up what we call a language.

With these considerations as a background of our consciousness, we may accept the statement that sound laws admit of no exceptions. Unless this were true, if, indeed, sound change were the result of chance or of whim, then, as Leskien said years ago (Deklination im Slavisch und Deutsch, 1877, p. xxviii), language, the subject of our investigations, would be incapable of scientific treatment, and there could be no science of language.

Sound laws are not of the nature of natural laws, since they have not a universal application to human language in general, but only hold good of a specific dialect at a given time. A sound law is merely a statement of a fact, or a sequence of facts, but does not include a statement of general conditions, under which these are bound to occur, nor an indication of the universal causes of the phenomena which are recorded.


CHAPTER VI

LINGUISTIC CONTACT

We have already seen how the speech of each individual within a given community presents certain characteristic personal peculiarities. Every individual speaker affects, and is affected by, the speech of every other speaker with whom he comes into contact. Similarly, the language of a small community influences, and is influenced by, the dialects, more or less closely related, of neighbouring communities.

This process of action and reaction of one form of speech upon another goes on wherever two or more individuals or communities are brought into social relations with each other. If it is traceable in the case of communities whose forms of speech are closely related, or are merely dialects of the same language, the effect produced by widely different, or totally unrelated languages, upon each other, is still more considerable.

The contact between two languages may be either direct, by personal intercourse between the speakers, or indirect, through the medium of literature. Direct contact comes about on the frontiers of two speech areas; by the transference of considerable communities among foreign races, either by a peaceful migration and settlement or through warlike invasion; or, again, by means of individuals who travel among foreign speakers, and sojourn for a greater or less period in another country.

The larger the number of speakers between whom and the foreign speakers contact exists, the greater the influence upon both languages. Colonization and conquest offer the most favourable conditions for linguistic contact on a considerable scale, provided that the new race does not drive out or exterminate the old. When two races live side by side, each preserving their own language, but, from the necessities of life, compelled to know, or at least to understand, that of the other to a certain extent, as in the case of the Scandinavians in England, who were first piratical invaders, then settlers, the influence of each language upon the other is likely to be profound. Under such conditions, there grows up in time, a large section, in both communities, which is bi-lingual. Perhaps at last the condition of bi-lingualism is reached by practically all speakers in each community. When this happens, one or other of the languages will gradually die out. The question of which community surrenders its language, will be determined by various social, intellectual, and other conditions. Intermarriage welds the two races into one, and the speech which survives as the language of the community, bears traces of that which has died out. The language which has gone under, may leave traces of its existence upon the pronunciation, the vocabulary, and the general structure of the language.

We have already pointed out that when a language is acquired by foreigners, the original pronunciation is never perfectly preserved, owing to the difference of the speech-bases. Although, here and there, an isolated individual DIRECT INFLUENCE—SOUNDS AND VOCABULARY may be able to speak two languages with equal perfection of pronunciation, this is impossible in the case of a large bi-lingual community. The speech basis of the native tongue is transferred to the newly-acquired language, and, as a result, the sounds of the latter undergo considerable modification. In the case where the native speech is acquired by the incoming race, it is maintained that the modification of this is far less than that which follows from the adoption of the immigrant language by the original inhabitants of a country (cf. Wechssler, Gibt es Lautgesetze? p. 97). The adoption of English by the Normans illustrates the former, that of the Romance languages by Teutons and Celts the latter.

The incorporation of any considerable proportion of foreign elements, into the vocabulary of a language, implies a certain amount of bi-lingualism—at least, for a time. A bi-lingual speaker will often introduce foreign words when speaking his own language, and vice versâ. At first, the words thus introduced from one language into another, are, chiefly, the designations of ideas or objects which are familiar to one people, but not to the other. The first reason for such loans is the actual necessity which is felt, to express a given conception, or to indicate some object for which no name exists in the language in use at the moment. The fact of a people possessing no name for a natural product does not imply any inferiority, though this may be inferred, up to a certain point, when the word borrowed is the name of some object of industry. On the other hand, the necessity of borrowing words which express ethical, religious, or political conceptions, most certainly denotes inferiority of moral and civil development, on the part of those who are compelled to seek their mode of expression from foreign sources. As a rule the new word is adopted at the same time as the idea, or the object which it denotes.

There are two ways of enriching the vocabulary of a language, when the need for this arises from the introduction of fresh ideas, or new products of human ingenuity: one, that which we have hitherto been considering, by incorporating new material from another tongue; the other, by adapting and combining elements of the native vocabulary, on the model of the foreign name. An example of this is the German vaterland or the Russian otíchestvo (atī́tʃɛstvo), which are translations of the Latin patria.

The introduction of foreign elements into a language in the first instance, usually starts, as we have seen, with an individual who is master of both tongues. In employing a foreign word, the individual has no intention to introduce a permanent element into the vocabulary: he merely supplies the necessity of the moment. For a word to become permanently fixed in a language, it is a necessary condition, as a rule, that it should be repeatedly used, and that it should be used spontaneously from several centres within the community. Foreign words gain a footing gradually. At first they are only used among a small group of individuals who are closely associated together by class, occupation, or nearness of geographical contiguity. Thence they may spread to other groups of a similar nature, and finally to the whole community. Some words may never come into general use, but may always be confined to the upper grades of the community. By the time a foreign element has passed into general LOAN-WORDS AND FOREIGN SOUNDS usage, it is no longer felt to be an alien, but has become part and parcel of the native language.

A foreign word generally gains currency in a form as near to the original as the natural pronunciation of the community permits. It is very rare that a word retains a sound which does not exist in the language into which it is borrowed. Still, foreign sounds are occasionally introduced into a language in isolated words, as, for instance, the initial (ž) of génie which is pronounced by the educated German, or the nasalized vowel in the French envelope which still survives in the pronunciation of some English speakers. Such foreign sounds, however, are confined to the more cultivated classes of a community, and in general use, the nearest sound in the native speech is substituted for them.

The original stress of foreign words is preserved long after their sounds have been replaced by the native sounds. Thus, while the numerous Norman-French words in Chaucer contain but few vowel or consonantal sounds which do not also occur in native English words, the original accent still persists in many, by the side however, of another form in which the accent is on the first syllable, as in English words—e.g., vertúe (Fr.), vértue (Eng.), licoúr and lícour, etc.[D]


Sounds which do not occur in native English words, but which were maintained in French loan-words, are: (oi) in joie, jointe, etc.; () probably still pronounced with slight nasalization in Chaucer’s day in chaunce, chaunge, etc. (tʃnsɛ, tʃndžɛ). Among consonants, the combination (dž) does not occur initially in English words, although common in Norman French: juge, gentil (džydzɛ, džɛntil), etc.

The Norman words which are found in English, won their way in through the prolonged direct, and intimate contact of the two races, which led to a final amalgamation. As the Normans were scattered throughout the length and breadth of the country, they affected all dialects equally. The Scandinavian invaders and settlers, on the other hand, were confined to certain districts. In those districts where they settled, the two races and the two languages were gradually fused; here the contact was direct and intimate. But the Scandinavian elements are not found in equal numbers in all dialects. In those dialects which had no direct contact with Scandinavian speech these elements are scanty, and when they exist, have spread from other areas where the influence of the Northmen was directly exercised. Thus foreign influence may pass indirectly to speakers who have had no direct contact with the alien race, through the medium of other speakers of their own blood, with whom the foreigners came into direct relation.

Still more attenuated, is the influence which one language may exert upon another through travellers, or others who spend some time in foreign countries, and then return to their own country, bringing accounts of strange customs or institutions, or articles of native industry. Many Indian words have passed into English through the intermediary of our civil and military officials. These words gain currency partly by means of literature, partly through direct contact of Anglo-Indians with their countrymen. The number of persons, among the governing classes in England, who have no connection with India through members of their family, or their friends is small, so that probably a very large number of Indian words have become SUBSTITUTION OF SOUND—INDIRECT INFLUENCE known to the upper classes of Englishmen, by word of mouth, from persons who acquired them direct from Indian speakers. On the other hand, the same words are known to other sections of the community in this country, only in their written form, from books and newspapers. Such words will be pronounced by the former class of persons with an approximation to their Indian form, and are thus in the same position as words acquired by direct contact; by the latter class, however, for whom they have never been living elements of a spoken language, they are uttered according to the nearest interpretation of the written symbols in harmony with their ordinary English values. Of course, as India and its institutions become more and more widely and directly known, the traditional pronunciation of Indian words obtains an ever-increasing diffusion.

The changes in pronunciation which words undergo in the process of their direct incorporation from living foreign languages, are in the nature of instantaneous substitution of the nearest native sound for the unfamiliar foreign sound. What are known as Acoustic changes, or changes due to faulty imitation, occur chiefly in foreign words. When once a word has been incorporated and thoroughly acclimatized, so that it is no longer felt as other than part of the language, it shares in all the changes of pronunciation which take place in the language.

We have now briefly to consider the influence of one language upon another as exerted through literature. When a foreign word gains a footing in a language, not from a living spoken tongue, but from one which is no longer spoken, which is dead, the only possible source from which it can come, is the written remains of the language as preserved in literature. The great culture languages of Greek and Latin have contributed, and continue to contribute, a large proportion of the vocabularies of every European language. Only next in importance, from this point of view is French, which, from the early Middle Age down to the present day, has been regarded as the chief vehicle among the modern languages of all that is distinguished and polite in Art and Letters. In the case of a living language, however, it is difficult to draw the line of distinction between influence which comes purely through the written form, and that which may be exerted directly by the uttered speech upon some individual or group, and which has spread from them, by word of mouth and by means of the pen, into the language of life and of literature. In the case of words borrowed from dead languages, however, there can be no doubt. Words from such a source acquire the sounds which in every respect are normal and natural in the language into which they are taken.

Many words borrowed from Latin into English are, and remain essentially, ‘learned’ as distinct from ‘popular’ words—that is to say, they belong to the language of books, and not to that of everyday life. We do not learn them as children in the ordinary course of social relations with our fellows, but acquire them later from our schoolmaster or our schoolbooks.

But many words which had a ‘learned’ origin pass, in the course of time, into universal usage in the language of everyday life; they are no longer felt as grand, important words, but express homely and familiar things or ideas. They cease LEARNED AND POPULAR WORDS—ARCHAISMS to be ‘learned,’ and become popular. It has been well pointed out that ‘the true distinction between a “learned” and a “popular” word depends not upon etymology, but upon usage’ (cf. Greenough and Kittredge, Words and their Ways in English Speech, p. 29). Such words as disaster, contradict, humour, are examples from among many, of words of distinctly learned origin, which are now in everybody’s mouth. Telephone, Telegraph, Phonograph, which are modern concoctions from the Greek, have come to be, owing to the progress of scientific and practical discovery, among the commonest words, just as the inventions which they designate are among the most familiar objects of modern life.

Another form of the process of borrowing words from a dead language is the revival of archaisms, or even of words which are completely obsolete, from earlier phases of the native language. This process is essentially artificial, and the old-new words rarely pass beyond the pages of the works in which their new birth takes place. At best, such revivals survive only in the mannered writing, or the painful and studied utterances of an individual, or of a literary clique.


CHAPTER VII

ANALOGY

The power of variously inflecting words in order to express different shades of thought and syntactic relations, comes naturally, in speaking a language of which we have even a moderate command. But such a power of ‘correctly’ forming adverbs from adjectives, of expressing past action, or plurality, or possession, does not depend upon the capacity of calling up the recollection of every individual form which is used. No human memory is stored with the past tenses of every verb which the speaker uses, with the comparative of every adjective, with the plural of every noun.

Nor is this necessary, for in the moment of utterance the formative element required, rises naturally in the mind of the speaker, although he may have no recollection of ever having heard it in that precise combination in which he is using it. The speaker, in fact, remakes for himself the conjugations of verbs, the declension of nouns, and so on, by the ‘correct’ use of certain formative suffixes. Were an effort of memory required in each instance, fluent and rapid speech would be impossible.

The fact is that comparatively few types remain in the memory, and from these the rest of the forms which the speaker uses are generalized, are made according to the model of those forms which actually are stored in the memory. This process is known as Analogy. Certain formative suffixes are associated in our minds with certain syntactic functions, and, as occasion demands, these inflexional elements, rise quite naturally into the consciousness, along with the shades of thought and meaning with which they are associated.

Analogy, and not memory for individual forms, is the natural process which takes place in the course of living utterance. The greater number of forms produced by this process are—allowing, of course, for the changes in sound which have occurred—identical with those which the same process called into existence at earlier periods of the language—that is to say, they are historically ‘correct.’ But in some cases new associations have been formed, so that the forms which a given generation of speakers, habitually, and naturally, call into existence in speaking, may differ from those which the speakers of earlier periods were in the habit of using.

The question of whether a form is ‘right’ or ‘wrong,’ is decided by the speech habit of the community at the time being. Forms in general use are ‘correct,’ those which are not in use are ‘wrong.’

An important point to bear in mind, however, is that, whether a form produced by a given speaker, by the process we are discussing, be ‘right’ or ‘wrong,’ in the sense in which we have just defined these terms, the actual process whereby the form is created, is the same in all cases. If a speaker makes use of a form which he has created according to some type which he has in his mind, but which is ‘wrong’ in the sense of not being the one in general use in the speech community of which he is a member, this arises from the fact that for some reason or other his associations, in this particular case, are different from those of the community at large.

The history of every language abounds with forms which are new departures from an earlier habit, and which are due to the formation of new association groups within the minds of the speakers of the generation which gave them birth. Words are associated in the mind, in groups, according to three main principles: their general affinity of meaning; identity of grammatical function; similarity of form. When more than one basis of association exists between a group of words, the association is doubly strong.

Examples of association by virtue of general affinity of meaning are—Natural Relationships: Father, Mother, Brother, Sister; the names of the seasons of the year: Spring, Summer, etc.; names of animals: (a) Wild Animals: Lion, Tiger; (b) Domestic Animals: Cat, Dog, Sheep, Oxen. In the same way we connect all the cases of an inflected substantive, all the persons and tenses of a verb, and so on. From this point of view, every word in the language naturally falls, in the mind of the speaker, into a group of words, linked together, more or less closely, by a general association of meaning. Such natural groups we may call association groups.

The second class of association groups, the members of which are linked together in our consciousness, are those whose basis of association is their community of grammatical or syntactical function. In this way are connected all plurals of substantives—dogs, boys, trees, etc.—which agree further in expressing the idea of plurality by the same formative element. Even when this is not the case, BASES OF ASSOCIATION—GRAMMATICAL FUNCTION and when the idea of plurality is expressed by different means, as in mice, houses, children, the association, though looser, still exists. Similarly, while all adverbs are associated as possessing a common function, the relations are of various degrees of closeness. In the most general way, simply as adverbs, hardly, well, here, are associated. But we can distinguish more intimately related groups of adverbs, such as adverbs of manner—hardly, bitterly, well, ill. Of these, the first two are peculiarly closely associated in possessing the same formative suffix—ly, and the last two have the further association of antithesis. Again, we may make an intimate group of adverbs of place—here, there, everywhere, and so on.

Passing to verbal forms, all preterites are associated in that they express the idea of past action—placed, told, rang, went, came. Within the large group of preterites, however, the weak past tenses, the strong past tenses, and the weak past tenses with change of vowel, form so many smaller and more closely related groups of association. Thus gave, came, wrote, are more nearly associated with each other than they are with sent, charmed, and so on. In the case of strong verbs there are small groups which have the same vowel sequence—sing, sang, sung; ring, rang, rung.

In speech, the way in which a past tense of a verb is formed, depends upon the associations which exist in the speaker’s mind. Thus, if a speaker had the association groups sing, sang, sung, ring, rang, rung, and fling, with past part. flung, he might quite naturally form a preterite *flang instead of flung. It would be incorrect to describe such a process as ‘false’ analogy, as is sometimes done. The actual process is ‘correct’ enough, although the result in this case is a form not commonly employed. The speaker who makes such a form, merely shows that he has not the past tense of fling in his memory, and that he forms one on the pattern of two other past tenses which happen to be the received forms. The ‘correct’ speaker who has heard the received form flung, has grown to isolate the word from the class of verbs which have the sequence of three vowels, and to form an association between it and such verbs as stick, stuck, and so on.

Whenever a speaker uses a form which strikes us as ‘wrong’—that, is unusual—we may be sure that there is some reason for it; and the interesting thing is to discover the precise association which exists in the speaker’s mind. If the association is different from that which exists in our mind, then the application of the principle of analogy, itself essentially the same in all cases, will lead to a different result.

The question of which is the ‘regular’ type within a given speech community depends partly upon the number of words which form the association group, and partly upon the frequency of occurrence. Sweet has pointed out (New Engl. Gr., § 538) that in colloquial language only common words, as a rule, present ‘exceptional’ forms. The plural men could never have been preserved had it been a word but rarely used. It is one of those isolated words which are, as it were, specially learnt at a very early age by constant repetition. But if the word man became obsolete, or fell into infrequent use, it is inevitable that we should form the plural according to the pattern of the thousands of other words in English which have -s-plurals. LEVELLING UNDER COMMONEST TYPE Young children, whose knowledge of, and experience in, the language is slight, constantly make such mistakes as ‘foots,’ ‘tooths,’ ‘oxes,’ and so on, simply because they have not learnt that these words are isolated from the vast majority of words which take -s-plurals.

Even in the case of common words, the attraction of larger groups often proves too strong, and the ‘exceptional’ forms tend to disappear. Thus we now say books, and in the standard language at any rate, cows, although O.E. had bēċ, which would have produced ‘beech’ in Mod. Eng., and , which would have given ‘ky’ (kai), which latter form, indeed, persists in Scotland and in some English dialects. Hence, it is frequently necessary to assume some additional association in order to explain the retention in Mod. Eng. of forms which differ from the common type. The O.E. neuter plural sce̅a̅p (Angl. scēp) persists in the modern plural ‘sheep’; and here we may perhaps assume an association with ‘flock’ or ‘herd,’ and regard a ‘flock of sheep’ as a kind of collective noun in which the individual animals are lost sight of. Another inevitable association of ‘sheep’ is with ‘cattle.’ We may contrast this view of sheep, en masse, with that of ‘lambs and their dams,’ when the comparative isolation of the individual mothers scattered over a field, with their offspring skipping round them, and the plurality of the individuals is forcibly brought home to the spectator.

A curious case is that of the plural fish applied chiefly to an article of diet, when the association is probably with ‘flesh’ or ‘food.’ This is a new plural, since the O.E. form was fiscas, and therefore demands the assumption of some new association such as that suggested. The form fishes, the descendant of the old plural, is applied more usually to the living creatures, especially when enumerating, or dealing with different species, as in the title of Couch’s famous book on British Fishes.

Words which constantly occur in the same phrase are often so closely associated in the mind that one suggests the other. Such pairs are: male and female; king and queen; mother and father; here, there, and everywhere; and so on. The reason, in the first place, for these phrases is that an intimate association of meaning exists between the words thus linked together. The result of such association is that the words influence each other formally. The word female is from an Old French femelle, Latin fēmella, which normally would appear in Mod. Eng., as (fīmɛl), a form heard in Scotch; but the association with male has influenced the second syllable, until many speakers believe the word to be a form of male with a prefix: hence the still further popular new formation ‘shemale,’ used jocularly.

In Scotch king is pronounced with a short, tense (i), the origin of which can scarcely be other than its association with queen (Scotch kwin). Mother in O.E. was mōdor, and the d continued into late M.E. The modern (ð) is undoubtedly due to the association with brother, O.E. brōðor, where the (ð) is original. The association between these two words is twofold—they both are names for family relationships, and they both have, and have always had, the same vowel. When once the open consonant was established in mother, this word influenced the word father, which in O.E. is fœder and in M.E. fāder and făder.

The pronunciations (ðīr, wīr) for there and where are ISOLATION FROM ORIGINAL ASSOCIATION-GROUP established for the eighteenth century (cf. Ellis, Early English Pronunciation, p. 104), and the same pronunciation of these words occurs in many popular dialects of the present day (cf. Wright’s English Dialect Grammar, under there and where in Index). It can hardly be doubted that we have here, not a normal phonetic development, but the result of the association of there and where with here, in which word the (ī) has arisen by regular sound change: (O.E. hēr, but hwœ̄r, þœ̄r).

A group of words of cognate origin are sometimes so far differentiated in form by different phonetic conditions that they cease to be felt as etymologically identical. In this case we say that a word has been isolated from its original association group. The words doom, -dom (in kingdom, etc.), and deem, are all derived from the same original root, dōm-, but probably no one but a student of the history of English associates them together in his mind at the present time. Deem, from O.E. dēman (vb.), shows a vowel changed by the process of i-mutation from an older ō, and -dom has sunk to the level of a mere formative suffix, and has no independent existence. From the substantive doom a new verb has been formed, which, however, has a different meaning from that of the original verb deem at the present time. It is generally the case that when two words have become isolated from each other by change of form, the meanings also grow further and further apart, till at last there is absolutely nothing which leads to an association between them. No English speaker now connects for-lorn with the verb lose, and yet the former was originally the regular past participle of the latter verb. The old verb forlose is lost except in the solitary surviving form just quoted, and the uncompounded verb lose has a newly-formed past participle, which is now, however, of some antiquity. The analogy of such a participle as for-sworn has maintained the fossil lorn; but its meaning has diverged considerably, and has grown further and further away from that of the simple verb lose, until there is nothing left, either in form or meaning, which should serve to connect them together in the mind of an ordinary speaker.

It often happens that before the association between a group or pair of words is quite broken by change of form, Analogy intervenes, and, eliminating some of the deviating forms, levels the group all under one type.

Take the words cool (adj.); to cool, coolness. Here O.E. has cōl, the normal ancestor of cool; but cēlan (vb.), and cēlnesse; (cf. dōm, dēman). In this case Analogy came into play in time to prevent a further differentiation of form and meaning, which might have broken all connection between the words, and has formed a new verb and a new abstract noun. The formal connection, as well as that of meaning, between these words and cold is possibly still felt by some speakers, but the association is not strong enough for them to affect each other formally. In the case of the further cognate chill, the association is probably entirely one of affinity of meaning. In the last case the differentiation is very far back indeed, and consists in a very primitive, pre-English difference of vowel and of formative suffix, and subsequent English combinative changes.

In cases where cognate forms which have been considerably differentiated by sound changes have resisted the DIFFERENTIATION OF MEANING tendency to isolate them from their original association group, as in the case of foot, which retains its plural feet, this is due, as has been said, to the frequency of occurrence, but also to the close association of general meaning which exists between the singular and plural of the same word.

It is sometimes said that Analogy hinders normal sound change, but this is scarcely accurate. What actually occurs is that, although the change is carried out regularly enough, yet, in certain cases, some stronger association works, with the result of re-creating a form identical with the old, on the analogy of some cognate which has not undergone the change. In such a case both forms, the new creation and that produced by the ordinary processes of sound change, are often preserved side by side, not infrequently, however, with a differentiation of meaning. The wider apart the two forms become, the greater the likelihood that each will be specialized for a different function. We have seen this to a certain extent in the two verbs deem and doom. Another case of a similar kind is seen in the two words ghostly and ghastly. The latter is the normal phonetic development of the O.E. adj. gāstlīċ, which in M.E. appears in the form găstlich(e) and găstli, with a normal shortening of O.E. ā before such a consonantal combination as -stl-. This word underwent a fronting of the vowel in the seventeenth century (gæstli). Then in the eighteenth (æ) was lengthened before -st-, giving a form (gǣstli), and this (ǣ) became (ā) in the late eighteenth or early nineteenth century. Ghostly, on the other hand, is a M.E. new formation from the substantive gōst, when the ō for O.E. ā is perfectly normal.

Another example of a similar process is seen in the adjectives formed by the suffix -like. This is originally cognate with the adjectival and adverbial suffix -ly, both being forms of the O.E. līc. The O.E. suffix is itself derived from the old substantive līċ = body, form. Thus originally wīflīc, ‘womanly,’ ‘feminine,’ meant ‘having the body or form of a woman.’ Already in O.E. when used as a suffix, the word had doubtless been completely isolated from the substantive in the consciousness of the speakers, and had become a mere formative element, although the association with ġelīċe, ‘like’ (literally ‘having the same form’), was probably still maintained. Then in M.E. the suffix -līk, -līch or -li, was shortened through lack of stress, became isolated even from ȝ̇elīch, ȝ̇elīk, and was still further emptied of its original independent meaning. When this had come about, a fresh class of adjectives arose, formed from -līk. Thus at the present time -ly, -like both exist as living suffixes, the former being principally adverbial, and we have the doublets wifely, wifelike, manly, manlike, and so on. The two suffixes, it will be noted, express different shades of meaning; the older being purely formative of adjectives or adverbs, the latter having the more definite sense of ‘like a wife’ or ‘beseeming a wife,’ etc. No doubt the association with the independent word like tends to preserve the diphthong (ai) even in the unstressed position.

The process of Analogy is operative in every period of linguistic development, and although attention is usually only called to it when it produces a new and strange form, it nevertheless comes into play in every utterance of connected speech. The history of any language shows that Analogy, besides working as a conservative factor by producing NEW ASSOCIATION GROUPS forms that are historically ‘correct,’ is also perpetually causing new departures, due to the gradual shifting of association groups which is ever taking place with every language which is alive, on the lips, and in the minds, of living speakers. These new associations are formed, in the first instance, within the individual consciousness, and their chance of becoming permanent parts of speech depends upon whether they are shared by the community at large. If this is not the case, the new departures of individual speakers are eliminated by social intercourse with that majority of other speakers who have different association groups. Just as each community has its own tendencies of sound change, which are different in some respects from those of other communities; so also each community has its association groups, which are different from dialect to dialect. When we come across a dialect whose speakers have a different series of associations from those which exist in our own minds, we are apt to consider the result as ‘ungrammatical’ and ‘wrong,’ forgetting that there is absolutely no test whereby we can gauge the inherent ‘correctness’ or ‘falseness’ of mental associations as expressed in speech. The human mind plays freely around and among the phenomena of speech; and we cannot control the subtle conditions which establish links between idea and idea, between word and word.

Within a given dialect certain associations are current, and practically universal, and therefore ‘correct’ so far as that dialect is concerned. The power to speak the dialect of a community ‘correctly’—that is, in the same way as the members of that community speak it—depends upon possessing the same association groups as they.

In tracing the history of a language, we are constantly confronted by forms which are the result, not of natural phonetic development, but of analogy, and in this case it is our business to endeavour to discover the group of forms with which the new association has been established. There is no limit to the period, nor to the dialect, in which these new formations arise; and experience teaches us that they did, as a matter of fact, come into existence and gain a permanent footing in the classical languages of antiquity, nay, in Primitive Aryan itself; just as they do at the present day, alike in polished literary speech, and in peasant dialect.


CHAPTER VIII

METHODS OF COMPARISON AND RECONSTRUCTION

The science of language is often divided into two main branches, General Comparative Philology of the Aryan languages (not to go beyond these for the moment), and the special History of the several Families of Aryan speech, or of individual languages. The Comparative Philologist, as such, is mainly concerned with that original unity which has been dissolved; with the original forms from which those of the various families and individual languages spring—that is, with the Primitive Aryan mother-tongue. The Comparative Philologist in the special sense is chiefly occupied with the reconstruction of this mother-tongue, and therefore is concerned primarily with the points of agreement between the different languages. But before he can reach the final unity, the primitive mother-forms, he must needs observe how great is the diversity among the groups of languages with which he deals; and this can only be accounted for from a knowledge of the special speech habits of the speakers of each language.

The investigation of these habits is the business of special students of the history of a single language, or of a group of closely allied tongues, such as the Germanic or Slavonic. By comparing the cognate forms of such a group, it is possible to form some idea of a phase of speech-life which is more primitive than any actually preserved—to reconstruct, in fact, Primitive Germanic or Primitive Slavonic.

But before we can compare words in different languages, with any profit, we must be quite sure that those forms we are comparing are really cognates—that they really are the descendants of the same original form. The closer the languages are in relationship, the less difficulty will there be in recognising their cognate forms. Thus the merest beginner would hardly doubt the affinity of O.E. fōt, ‘foot,’ Gothic fōtus, O.Norse fōtr, O.H.G. fuoz. Even if he went further, and ascertained that ‘foot’ in Scrt. was pād-, pad-, in Greek πούς, in Latin pēs he might surmise that these were all forms of the same word which is found in the Germanic languages. The tests of identity of origin, are form and meaning. But, since related languages often develop on widely differing lines, the form frequently undergoes very remarkable changes, and the meaning may vary so greatly, that it is not always easy to see how this or that particular shade of significance becomes attached to a particular root.

The science of Comparative Philology has been gradually built up, until we are now often able to assert with confidence, the original identity of words, which, a few years ago, no one would have dreamed of connecting with each other. This is made possible by our ever-increasing knowledge of the laws of sound change within the individual languages. By this means it is possible gradually to divest a form of its more recent peculiarities, and to reconstruct its earlier phases, so that many old friends emerge, as it were, from TESTS OF IDENTITY disguise. But in the beginning it was necessary to start with such words as from their nature, admitted but little change in meaning, and whose form in several tongues was sufficiently recognised to prohibit any reasonable doubts of identity. The classes of words most suitable for purposes of comparison, in the beginning, are words which express concrete and familiar objects, such as the natural relationships—father, mother, brother, etc.; names of parts of the body—head, eyes, ears, feet, etc.; names for the earth, the sky, water, the wind, heat, cold, snow; names of the most widely distributed plants and animals. Further, we should expect to find the designation of the numerals, at any rate up to ten, the common property of men whose ancestors had, in ages however remote, spoken one and the same language. These are the kind of words upon which the foundations of Comparative Philology are laid, and when these are built with care and thoroughness, the way is paved for further progress. Now, when, in the case of words in different languages of whose identity there can be no reasonable doubt, even from the beginning, we observe a regular permutation of sounds constantly recurring throughout a series of languages, when the differences between the languages are always of the same nature, we are able to lay it down as a general principle, based on observation, that such and such a sound in this language corresponds with such and such a sound in that. We proceed upon the assumption that the same changes will always occur, under the same conditions, in the same language; if we find in a large number of cases that when Greek, Latin, etc., have p, Germanic shows f, we expect that this will always be the case, when the conditions are the same. In those cases where Greek p does not correspond to f in Germanic, we assume, either that the p in question does not represent the same original sound as that which we know becomes f in Germanic, or that there are conditions present which differentiate the case from others with which we are familiar. These conditions it then becomes our business to discover.

We do not believe that Greek and Latin are derived from Sanscrit; nor Germanic from Greek or Latin; but rather, that they are all derived from a common ancestor now long dead. Therefore, we do not state our sound law in the form of saying that Sanscrit, Greek, and Latin p becomes f in Germanic; but that a Primitive Aryan p is retained in the former three languages, but has become f in Germanic. Having gained, then, some knowledge of the precise way in which the groups of languages we are comparing, agree with, or differ from each other, and, further, a knowledge of some of the principal laws of sound-change of each of the derived languages, we ask what were the original forms from which those forms which we know have developed. In other words, the question we try to solve is, which of the forms before us is most primitive, which preserves most faithfully the features of the original common mother. The reconstructed forms of Primitive Aryan or Primitive Germanic which, according to present philological method, figure so largely in comparative and historical studies must not be taken too seriously therefore; these merely record the opinion that this or that feature in this or that language is primitive and original and in assigning such and such a form as the common ancestor of a group of forms from various ‘SEEK’ AND ‘BE-SEECH’ languages we must be prepared to show how each is derived from it.

In tracing the history of a word, root, or grammatical form in a single language, we get, as a rule, more light upon it the further we can go back; and by allowing for the various isolative and combinative sound changes which have affected it, we are gradually able to show the original identity of the root with that which occurs in a considerable number of words. But so long as we keep to one language we can only discover the principle of those changes the conditions of which were present at some time during the period of which we have an historical record of that language. Thus if we were dealing with the history of the word seek in English compared with be-seech, we should first inquire what was the oldest recorded form of these words. A glance at an etymological dictionary, or, better still, at an ‘Anglo-Saxon’ dictionary, would reveal the fact that in both cases the infinitive was sēc(e)an, with nothing to show that the present difference between the final consonants of the two words existed. In Middle English we find that sēken, sēchen, besēken, besēchen, all occurred; and, further, that in the present-day English dialects seek, seech, beseek, beseech, are in use in different parts of the country. Now, the Mod. Eng. ‘ch-’ (tʃ) sound presupposes a different sound in O.E. from that which has become k in Mod. Eng., and that sound, we should find, if we consulted an O.E. grammar, was certainly pronounced in the O.E. sēc(e)an. It was probably a front-stop consonant, and it invariably develops into the Mod. Eng. ‘-ch’ (tʃ); at any rate, in the South and Midlands. At this rate the M.E. sēchen would appear to be normally developed from O.E. sēċ(e)an. How are we to account for the M.E. and Mod. Eng. forms with -k? Certainly not by assuming an ‘exceptional’ change of (front-stop) to (k). If we look at the paradigm of the O.E. verb, it appears that in West Saxon it ran as follows in the Pres. Indic. Sing.: iċ sēċe, þu secst, hē secþ; and in M.E. the same texts which have ich sēche in 1st person singular, and sēchen in the Inf., not infrequently have sēkst, sēkþ in the 2nd and 3rd persons. The O.E. spelling does not express any difference of pronunciation; but the M.E. spelling shows a back-stop in the two last forms, and this implies a corresponding distinction in O.E., although this is not expressed in the written forms of that language. What conditions have these two forms in common, which distinguish them from the 1st Pers. and from the Inf.? They both have voiceless open consonants, s and þ respectively, immediately after the ċ. May we not, then, formulate tentatively the law that in O.E., before ċ had developed into its present sound,—perhaps even before it had reached the pure front-stop stage,—when it was followed immediately by a voiceless open consonant, it became a back-stop (k)? This is borne out by other examples. We have thus accounted for the existence of two forms with k-sounds in the conjugation of the O.E. verb sēċan. But we have still to explain how this sound got into the 1st Pers. Pres. Indic. and the Inf.

We are perfectly justified, from what is known of the habits of speakers, in assuming the possibility that a whole verb might be formed on the Analogy of two persons, especially when these are so frequently used as were the 2nd and 3rd persons singular in O.E. and M.E. We ‘SEEK’ AND ‘SOUGHT’ should explain M.E. sēken, etc., and Mod. Eng. seek in this way. For some reason the analogy has not taken place in be-seech, which retains the O.E. ċ- form unaffected by the other persons. In the case of the dialects above referred to, the Analogy affects sometimes the compounded, sometimes the uncompounded verb.

This digression from the general statement is intended to show that reference to the earlier forms of a language may tell us something which cannot be gathered from its latest forms. The varying conditions which subsequently differentiated O.E. ċ into k on the one hand, and on the other to ‘-ch’ (tʃ), were present, and expressed in the spelling of English itself. But if we now proceed to inquire the reason of the differences of vowel between seek or seech, on one hand, and that of the past tense sought, on the other, we can get no light, so long as we confine our attention to English. As far back as we can go in the history of that language, we find this difference of vowels, but nothing to account for it. O.E. has sēċansōhte, and here we can note that the variation is ēō, an interchange which occurs in a large number of associated pairs of words in O.E., it is true; but this fact does not help us to explain the change.

The next step, therefore, is to inquire what is the corresponding form to O.E. sēcan in the other Gmc. languages. It is possible that some of these may retain some feature which O.E. has lost, and which may explain the interchange of vowels. The corresponding verb in Gothic is sōkjan, in O. Sax. sōkian, in O.H.G. suohhan. From these forms we learn that O.E. is peculiar in having ē in the root of the Inf. It appears that both Gothic and O. Sax. have ō, which vowel, as we have seen, also occurs in O.E. in the Pret. O.H.G. uo appears in a large number of words in which Gothic and O. Sax. have ō. We are, therefore, justified in assuming that ō is the most primitive form of the vowel in the inf. Why has O.E. ē here? Now, both Gothic and O. Sax. possess a feature which does not appear either in O.H.G. or in O.E., and that is that they preserve a suffix -jan or -ian in the inf.; that is to say that j or i appears in these languages immediately after the k. The sound of j, we have reason to believe, was that of a front-open consonant, closely related, from the position of the organs of speech and the area employed in its articulation, to i, which is a high-front vowel. Now, -jan is a very common verbal suffix in Gothic, and in all cases where O.E. and Gothic agree in possessing certain verbs, we find that the vowel of these verbs, if ō in Gothic, is ē in O.E.; if a in the former language, e in the latter; if ū̆ in Gothic, then ȳ̆ in English—that is, that where Gothic has a back vowel English shows a front in the inf. of corresponding verbs, when there is reason to believe that a j originally occurred in the suffix. For example; Goth. drōbjan, ‘disturb,’ ‘trouble,’ O.E. drēfan; Goth, fōdjan, ‘feed,’ O.E. fēdan; Goth, ga-mōtjan, ‘meet,’ O.E. mētan, and so on. Examples of Goth. a = O.E. e, under the same conditions, are: Goth, namnjan, ‘name,’ O.E. nemnan; Goth, satjan, ‘set,’ O.E. settan; Goth. warjan, ‘defend,’ O.E. werian. Examples of Goth. u = O.E. y are: Goth. bugjan, ‘buy,’ O.E. byċġan; Goth. fulljan, ‘fill,’ O.E. fyllan; Goth. huggrjan (= huŋgrjan), ‘to hunger,’ O.E. hyngr(i)an. In all these cases Gothic shows consistently a back vowel ANCESTRAL FORMS OF OLD ENGLISH in the root, followed by j; O.E. invariably has in the same words a front vowel in the root, but has usually no j or i following. We need not pause here to discuss under what circumstances j is also preserved in O.E., but may note that when it is lost in that language the preceding consonant is doubled, provided that the sound immediately preceding the consonant is not a long vowel (cf. settan and byċġan, where ċġ is the O.E. mode of writing a long voiced stop).

In all the above cases, although only Gothic forms are here given, O. Sax. and O.H.G. agree in showing ō (O.H.G. uo), a, and u respectively where O.E. has ē, e, and y. The inference we draw is that ō, a, and u are more primitive than the English vowels in these words, and that the special quality of these, front instead of back, is due to a change in the earlier sounds produced by the following j or i. This is still further borne out by the fact that ō, etc., are preserved in O.E. itself, in cases where the root is not followed by j or i. Thus by the side of mētan we have in O.E. the substantive ġe-mōt, by the side of fēdan, fōda, ‘food,’ just as we have sōh-te by the side of sēċ(e)an. With O.E. nemnan we may compare the sub. nama, and with fyllan the adj. full. The comparison of the other Germanic tongues, in deciding the question of the difference of vowel in sēċ(e)ansōhte, showed us that O.E. must also once have had an inf. *sōkjan, since it enabled us to supply the lost j which effected the change from the more primitive vowel ō, preserved in Gothic and O. Sax. The forms in the cognate languages also made it certain that the original vowel was the same as that preserved in the unchanged forms in O.E. itself. Another fact which emerges from our examination of the above forms is that the particular change in question, which has already been referred to in an earlier chapter of this book, although it took place before the earliest English documents, yet occurred after English had developed into a dialect, or group of dialects, independent from the parent Germanic. Had the change affected Primitive Gmc. before its differentiation, we should find traces of it in Gothic; whereas we find none, and only signs of its beginning in O. Sax. and O.H.G. This process of i- or j-mutation, as it is called, arose independently in English, and, at a later date, in most of the other Gmc. languages. It affects all back vowels in O.E. which occur in the roots of words containing originally j or i in the next syllable or suffix; not only in verbs, as in the examples given above, but in all words whose suffix fulfils, or once fulfilled, the necessary conditions.

When once the knowledge of such a process has been gained by a comparison of the cognate languages, it can be utilized for purposes of reconstruction, without a further appeal to the comparative method. Thus, if we find the O.E. forms betst, ‘best,’ fyrst, ‘first,’ compared with fur-đor, we should be justified in assuming the possibility of an old superlative suffix -ist, which has changed a and u to e and y in these words, even if we had not, for the moment, the confirmatory evidence of Gothic bat-ist-s, ‘best.’

We see that a knowledge of the sound changes peculiar to the individual languages helps us to reconstruct primitive forms which may be of use in a wider comparative survey; but this special knowledge of an individual GOTHIC TUNÞUS AND OLD ENGLISH TOÞ language can only be gained, at first, by knowing what was the starting-point of the language we are considering, and this knowledge, again, can only be acquired with certainty by the help of the cognate languages. Our Primitive Gmc. forms, which we may reconstruct from English alone, must be tested by comparing them with the other Gmc. languages. If from our knowledge of the laws of each, we reach the same result in reconstruction, no matter from which we start, then we may have a very fair conviction that our reconstruction is right.

But it sometimes happens that the consideration of the Gmc. languages alone leaves us in the lurch, and that we are stopped by what are insuperable difficulties, so far as the light shed from these alone reaches.

If, for instance, we compare the Gmc. forms of so common a word as ‘tooth,’ we find that in O.E. we have tōþ, in Goth. tunþus, in O.H.G. zand; and we may well ask what is the relation of these forms to each other. Gothic and O.E. agree in the initial and final consonants of the root t and þ; there is, therefore, the a priori reason of greater frequency, for assuming that t and þ are more primitive than the O.H.G. z and d. On the other hand, Gothic and O.H.G. agree in having a nasal consonant after the vowel, and we must assume either that O.E. has lost an n, or that Gothic and O.H.G. have both introduced one in this word. According to the same general principle of relative frequency of occurrence, it is more reasonable to assume that these languages preserve an original nasal here, where O.E. has lost it. It is improbable that two languages so far separated geographically as Gothic and O.H.G., should have developed, independently, a habit of infixing nasals. We naturally next inquire why, in this case, O.E. has lost an original nasal which is preserved by Gothic and O.H.G. There are plenty of examples of words in which the latter languages have a nasal, but in which O.E. has not: O.H.G. gans, ‘goose,’ O.E. gōs; Goth. munþs, O.H.G. mund, ‘mouth,’ O.E. mūþ; Goth. sinþs, ‘road,’ ‘journey,’ O.H.G. sind, also Goth. ga-sinþja, O.H.G. gi-sindo, ‘travelling companion,’ ‘servant’; O.E., sīþ, ġe-sīþ, Goth. anþar, O.H.G. andar, ‘other,’ O.E. ōþer; Goth. and O.H.G. hansa, ‘host,’ O.E. hōs; O.H.G. samfto, ‘soft,’ O.E. sōft. These examples suffice to show the conditions under which the nasal is lost in O.E. It will be observed that in all the above cases, there is in Gothic, immediately after the nasal, and in O.E., following the vowel, one or other of the three consonants, s, f, or þ—that is to say, a voiceless open consonant.

The agreement of Gothic and O.E., as regards the consonants, is a strong indication of these being primitive, so that we can formulate the law that O.E. loses a nasal (n, or m) before voiceless open consonants, and we can reconstruct for prehistoric O.E., forms with the nasals as they occur in Gothic.

It is further to be noticed that the vowel which precedes the nasal undergoes in O.E. a compensatory lengthening, and that in cases where Gothic and O.H.G., and therefore presumably the parent Gmc. also, have the combination -an + voiceless open consonant, O.E. has ō—that is to say that in this case, the original a has been rounded as well as lengthened. We may now return to O.E. tōþ, and in the light of the above examples and *TAN-Þ AND *TUN-Þ BOTH PRIMATIVE remarks, we see that we shall be justified in reconstructing therefrom an earlier form *tanþ-, which, allowing for the regular differences of the consonants, agrees entirely with the O.H.G. zand. The Gothic form, on the other hand, as we have seen, is tunþ-us instead of tanþ-, as we might have expected on the analogy of anþar compared with O.E. ōper.

Is there any process of change peculiar to Gothic whereby a form tanþ- could become tunþ-? There is none; and the Gothic forms with -un-, such as munþs, quoted above, and kunþs, ‘known,’ O.E. cūþ, O.H.G. chund; juggs (=juŋg-), ‘young,’ O. Fris., O.S., O.H.G. jung; hund, ‘hundred’; O.E., O.Sax. hund, O.H.G. hunt, etc., show that Gothic, as a rule, agrees with the other Gmc. languages in preserving the combination -un- in cognate words. Indeed, the agreement is so complete, and so widely extended among the Gmc. languages; that, following the ordinary method, we must assume that Gmc. -un- is preserved in all the languages; and, conversely, that when the derived languages all agree in showing this combination it is original. The result of this is that we must regard the Gothic form tunþ- as original: preserved from the parent language, and not derived from any other form of the same ‘root.’ We are therefore compelled to conclude that there were in Gmc. two forms of this root: one, tunþ-, preserved in Gothic, and another, *tanþ-, from which the O.E. and O.H.G. forms, and the O. Norse tannr, from *tanþ-r, from *tanþ-az, were derived. How are we to account for the differentiation of an original ‘root’ into two forms, *tanþ- and tunþ-? The fact itself is common enough in Gothic and the other Gmc. languages, and the so-called strong verbs offer plenty of examples. The following table will illustrate this:

InfPret.Sing.Pret. Pl.Past Partic.
     
O.E.bind-anbandbund-onbund-en‘bind’
Goth.bind-anbandbund-umbund-an-s
O.H.G.bint-anbantbunt-umbunt-an
     
O.E.wind-anwandwund-wund-‘wind’
Goth.-wind-anwandwund-umwund-ans
O.H.G.vinn-anvantvunt-umvunt-an
     
O.E.winn-anwannwunn-wunn-‘struggle’
Goth.-winn-anwannwunn-umwund-ans
O.H.G.vinn-anvannvunn-umvunn-an

Numerous examples also occur of the same ‘root’ appearing in different forms.

Gothic has -hinþ-an, ‘to catch,’ hand-us, ‘the hand,’ originally ‘that which seizes,’ and hunþ-s, ‘that which is seized,’ or ‘booty’; O.E. has hand, and hūþ, ‘booty,’ from *hunþ-, with the loss of the nasal before -þ-, as in mūþ, from *munþ-; O.H.G. hant, ‘hand,’ and heri-hunda (= O.E. hūþ), ‘war plunder.’ Side by side with sinþs and ga-sinþa, Goth. has the vb. sand-jan, ‘send,’ and O.E. sīþ > *sinþ- and send-an > *sand-jan, with the j-mutation of a referred to above. Besides the changes which occur in the strong vb. bindan, Gothic has and-bund-nan, ‘to release’; bandi, ‘a fetter’ (exactly corresponding to O.E. bend, where e is the i-mutation of a); and ga-binda, ‘bond,’ etc.

These examples show that this interchange of vowels within the same ‘root’ was an established fact in Gmc. before its differentiation, since it occurs in all the derived languages. We can, therefore, learn nothing of its origin LIGHT SHED BY WIDER COMPARISON from Gmc. alone. If we go beyond Gmc., and compare the forms in the other Aryan languages which are cognate with tunþus, etc., we find a curious variety of forms. Latin dent-, Gk. ὀ-δόντ-, Scrt. dant-, Lith. dant-ìs, are the forms in the principal Aryan languages which we have to compare with each other, and with the two Gmc. types *tanþ- and tunþ-, which we have found ourselves justified in reconstructing. The question now before us is: What are the Primitive Aryan types from which the above forms are derived, and what is their precise mutual relationship? Our comparison of the Gmc. languages yielded two types for parent Gmc.; to what does a wider survey lead us? In the first instance, we may settle the question of the consonants. We note that Scrt., Gk., Latin, and Lith. all agree in having d- as the initial, and -t- as the final consonant of the root; and in the face of this unanimity we must conclude that sounds which all these languages have preserved, are the original Aryan sounds. Gmc. t = original d-, and þ = original t, are the result of a characteristic ‘shifting’ of the older consonants, which, with the reservation formulated in what is known as Verner’s Law, hereafter to be discussed, invariably produces the same results; so that wherever the other languages agree in having d, Gmc. has t, and where they have t, Gmc. has þ, except under the special conditions stated by Verner.

We may now return to the vowels, and for this purpose it will be convenient to deal here with the group of vowel + n,—on, en, an, etc. It might be contended that since Scrt., Lith., and Gmc. all agree in possessing a form of the above root with -an-, this must be regarded as a primitive form; let us see whether this can be upheld. If -an- is to be regarded as a primitive Aryan form, it can only be on account of the agreement in the three languages which we have just noted. This assumption would imply that we regard a primitive -an- as having been preserved in Scrt., Lith., and Gmc. We shall do well to examine severally the claims of each language to the primitiveness of its -a- and -an- sounds. Let us take Scrt. first. Although this language agrees with Gmc. and Lith. in this case, it is at variance with Gk., which has -ον-. The same disparity is observable in Scrt. jambha-, ‘tooth’; Gk. γόμφος, γομφίος, ‘molar’ (which correspond to O.E. camb, ‘comb’), and in tam, ‘this’ (acc.); Gk. τόν; Goth. þan-a; Scrt. damas, ‘house’; Gk. δόμος; Lat. domus. Here we have Scrt. and Gmc. an, am by the side of Gk. -ον-, -ομ-.

But in Scrt. janas, ‘race,’ we have -en- both in Latin and Gk.—genus, γένος; and the same divergence appears in Scrt. bandhus, a ‘relative,’ compared with Gk. πενθερός. Lith. also shows disagreement with Scrt. here, for its cognate is bèndras, ‘companion.’ This is the same root which in Gmc. has, as we have seen, the three forms bind-, band-, bund-. In Scrt. ánti, ‘against,’ Gk. ἀντὶ, Lat. ante, Scrt. agrees with Gk. and Latin.

These examples show that Scrt. -an- is represented in Gk. sometimes by -ον-, sometimes by -εν-, more rarely by αν-.

If we compare the correspondences of simple a in Scrt. without a following nasal, we find the same divergence in some, at least, of the cognate languages.

1. Scrt. a = Gk. α in ájami, ‘drive’; Gk. ἄγω, Lat. ago: CORRESPONDENCES OF SCRT. A IN GK. AND LATIN ajras, ‘ground’; Gk. αγρός; Lat. ager; Goth. akrs.

2. But Scrt. a = Gk. ο in pati, ‘husband’; Gk. πόσις: avi-, ‘sheep’; Gk. ὄις (from *ὄϝις); Lat. ovis: katara, ‘which of two’; Gk. πότερος: dadarš́a, ‘he has seen’; Gk. δέδορκε, etc.

3. Scrt. a = Gk. ε in asti, ‘is’; Gk. ἐστί; Lat. est; Lith. ẽsti.

Scrt. aš́va, ‘horse’; Lat. equus: Scrt. ca, ‘and’; Gk. τὲ; Lat. que.

Scrt. páta-ti, ‘he flies’; Gk. πέτε-ται; Lat. petit, etc.

We see that the three vowels a, e, o in Latin and Greek are all represented in Sanscrit by a; in fact, e and o do not exist at all in this language. If, then, Scrt. a be in all cases primitive, we must assume that the other languages which possess a more varied vowel system have differentiated an original vowel a into three distinct sounds, a, e, o. The alternative is that the three vowels existed in the mother-tongue, but were all levelled in Scrt. under one sound, a.

Passing to Lithuanian, this language agrees with Scrt. in having a where Gk. and Latin show o: nakt-is, ‘night,’ Lat. nox (= *nokt-s); -patis, ‘lord’; Gk. πόσις; avis, ‘sheep’; Gk. ὄ(ϝ)ις, Lat. ovis.

On the other hand, Lithuanian agrees with Gk., Lat., Gmc. in showing e, thus differing from Scrt.—esmi, ‘am’; Gk. ειμι (= εσμι): medùs, ‘honey’; Gk. μεθυ; O.E. medu (= *medu); O.H.G. metu; but Scrt. madhu: sẽnas, ‘old’; Gk. ἕνος (= *σένος); Lat. senex. Again, the closely-allied Slavonic languages, such as Old Bulgarian (or Old Church Slav.), agree also with Gk. in having o in cases where Lith. has a: O. Slav. nosti, ‘night’; Lith. naktis. O. Slav. ovi-tsa, ‘sheep’; Lith. avis. This makes it probable that o existed in Primitive Lith. also, but was unrounded to a in the independent life-history of the language.

Last we have to deal with Germanic, which, like Scrt., had already, in its earliest literary period, no original o sound; at any rate, not in stressed syllables. It can be shown that when this vowel appears in the Old Gmc. languages, it is either derived by a secondary process from an earlier u, or has been preserved in late loan words from foreign languages. In all cases where Gk. has ο, Gmc. has a in cognate words. But it can be established that the sound o underwent a change to a within the historic period, since foreign proper names which contained the former sound appear in Gmc. speech, when borrowed, with a. Thus the Gallo-Roman Moguntiacum, ‘Mainz,’ is Maginza in O.H.G.; and Vosegus, ‘the Voges,’ appears with a in O.H.G., as Wascono walt. The inference generally drawn from these facts is that up to a certain period, parent Gmc. preserved o, which it inherited from Aryan; but that then a tendency arose to unround o to a, which tendency naturally affected the loan words also. Those words which were borrowed subsequent to this change, preserved their o-sound in Gmc. speech (cf. O.H.G. kocchōn, ‘to cook,’ from Lat. coquere).

If the above reasoning be correct, then Gmc. originally possessed the vowel o; its a is not primitive in those cases where it corresponds to o in Gk. and Latin, and therefore proves nothing when compared with the a of Scrt. and Lith.

We have now briefly examined the claims of a in Scrt., Lith., and Gmc. successively, to be regarded as primitive in cases where Gk. and Latin have the vowel o. We have seen that Scrt. a corresponds not only to a in Gk. and ‘PALATIZATION’ IN SCRT. Latin, but also to e and o; and we are therefore forced to admit, either that Gk. and Latin preserve the three original sounds, or, at any rate, an original diversity, whereas Scrt. has lost it; or that in the former languages, one original sound, without any discoverable difference of conditions, has been treated in three different ways. The latter possibility we may reject at once on general grounds. For the former view there are overwhelming arguments. Of these, that which establishes beyond any reasonable doubt the primitiveness of Gk. e, is the strongest; and to it is due the conviction, now universally shared by all philological scholars, that the Gk. vowel system is far nearer to that of the original Aryan than are the Sanscrit vowels.

There are certain words which have a variety of back-stop in Latin, Celtic, and Lithuanian, but which in Sanscrit have a sound, expressed in transliteration by the symbol c, and usually pronounced (tʃ), but which is classified as a ‘palatal,’ and was originally, almost certainly, a front-stop. The vowel which follows it is always a in Scrt. In Gk. these words have π or τ, which, for reasons into which it is needless to enter here, are known to have developed from a back-stop with lip modification.

This ‘palatalization’ in Sanscrit was for a long time unaccounted for, since, in other words, Sanscrit agrees with the languages above mentioned in also having k—that is, a back consonant.

The explanation was discovered independently by several scholars about the same time (see Bechtel, Hauptprobleme, p. 62). It is this: In cases where the European languages (Gk., Latin, etc.) have a or o following the consonant. Sanscrit agrees with them in having a back consonant; in those cases where the former languages have e, Sanscrit has c, the front consonant. A natural inference is that in Sanscrit also, e formerly occurred in those cases where it is found in Gk., Latin, etc., and, e being a front vowel, fronted the preceding consonant. After the fronting process was complete, Sanscrit levelled e under a, the series of changes probably being: eœa. If this is so, then prehistoric Sanscrit must have agreed with all the European tongues in possessing e, and thus the last argument against accepting this as the original sound disappears.

Examples are: Scrt. panca, ‘five,’ Gk. πέντε (from *penkwe); Lat. quinque (from *kwenkwe, from *penkwe). Scrt. catvấras, ‘four,’ Gk. τέσσαρες and πέσσαρες (Bœotian), Lith. keturi, Old Irish cethir. On the other hand, Sanscrit has kákša, ‘hip-joint’ = Lat. coxa; also kakúd, ‘summit’ = Lat. cacūmen.

When it was thus established that Sanscrit a was not original in cases where the other languages had e, it was further asked, Why should Scrt. a, which corresponds to o in Gk. and Lat., etc., be original either? No reason could be shown for the development in these languages of o from an earlier a; but, on the other hand, belief in the primitiveness of the Scrt. vowel system was seriously shaken. Henceforth, it was regarded as, at the very least, highly probable that the three vowels a, e, o all existed in the Aryan mother-tongue; a view which, as has been said, scholars now regard as established. Of all the Aryan languages, the Hellenic group are now considered to preserve the primitive vowel system most faithfully. Greek is by far the richest in vowel sounds, and hence, instead of attributing, as was formerly done, a poor vowel system to the mother-tongue, SUMMARY OF RESULTS it is now the universal practice to credit it rather with the wealth and variety which is found in that group of dialects, than with the poverty and comparative monotony of Sanscrit.

After this long discussion, which it is hoped may have afforded some illustration of the methods of comparison and reconstruction, we may return to a consideration of the various forms of the root ‘tooth’ in the different Aryan languages.

We had established (see p. 154) the existence of two forms of the root in Gmc.—*tunþ-, which is found in Gothic, and *tanþ-, which is the ancestor of O.E. tōp and O.H.G. zand. The forms enumerated from other languages were—Scrt. dant; Lith. dant-ìs; Lat. dent-; and Gk. ὀ-δόντ-. From what has just been said, it will be seen that we are now in a position to regard Gk.-δοντ- as primitive, and practically identical with the ancestral form. We are further justified in equating it with the Gmc. *tanþ (see p. 158), and with the Lith. dant-ìs (pp. 157, 158).

As regards the Scrt. form, the a might represent either an original o, in which case the Scrt. form may also be derived from the form *dont-, or it might be derived from an earlier *dent-. Since, however, the former is so well established for several branches of the Aryan family, it is on the whole, perhaps, more probable that the Scrt. form also goes back to this, in common with Lith., Gk., and Gmc. We may now pass on to discuss the Latin form dent- and the Gothic tunþ-us. What are the mutual relations of these, and what connection have they with the Aryan *dont- which we have established?

Lat. dent- might, if taken by itself, be an original form, representing an Aryan *dent-; just as Gk. πενθ-ερός, Lith. bend-ras, represent an original *bhendh-. This form occurs in Gmc. as bind-an, with Gmc. change from e to i before n + consonant. At this rate, original *dent- would produce in Gmc. *tenþ-, and thence *tinþ-, but this form of this particular word is not found in any Gmc. tongue.

There are other cases, however, when Lat. -en corresponds to Gmc. -un: for instance, Lat. cent-um, Goth. hund-, ‘100’; to these forms there correspond ἑ-κατόν in Gk., szimtas in Lith., and š́atám in Scrt. Again, Lat. ment-, ‘mind’; Goth. ga-mund-s, ‘remembrance,’ corresponds to Scrt. mati-, ‘thought.’ In these cases we see that Lat. en, Gmc. un, correspond to forms in Scrt. and Gk. which have no nasal. In this case Lat. en cannot be derived from an original en, since, as we have just seen, that is preserved in Gk. and in Scrt. becomes an (πενθερός, Lat. of-fendix, ‘tie,’ ‘band’; Scrt. bandhus, etc.); further, original en equals Gothic -in-, and not -un-. We may formulate our results so far thus:

{Scrt.-an-}
{Gk.-ον-}
The Series{Lat.-on-}= Idg. -on-.
{Gmc.-an-}
    
{Scrt.-an-}
{Gk.-εν-}
The Series{Lat.-en.}= Idg. en.
{Gmc.-en (in)}
    
{Scrt.a}
{Gk.-α-}
The Series{Lat.-en-}= Idg. ?
{Gmc.-un-}

That is to say that by the side of the forms -en- and -on- of roots with a nasal, we must assume that a third form existed—a form which, whatever it was, acquired various sounds in the separate development of each Aryan language. It is generally assumed that this third form was a weakened form which possessed, originally, no definite vowel sound, VOCALIC N, M INARYAN but contained a syllabic nasal very similar, probably, to the second syllable of the English word ‘button’ (batn). Comparative philologists usually write this hypothetical sound , to distinguish it from the consonantal n, or in the case of centum, etc.; cf. Lith. szimtas, from Aryan *ḱm̥tóm. We have thus established a strong probability that Gothic tunþ- and Latin dent- are both from an original form *dn̥t-, whereas the various other forms of this word, including the O.H.G. zand and O.E. tōþ, are all derivable from a primitive *dont-.

Although only two forms of this root have survived, other similar roots preserve all three forms, thus: πενθερός, bendras and bind-, from *bhendh-; band and bandhus, from *bhondh; bund and of-fend-ix, from *bhn̥dh-. This differentiation of an original vowel, which goes back to the mother-tongue, is known as Ablaut or Gradation. The supposed causes of this phenomenon will be treated later on.

We have endeavoured in the above discussion to illustrate the method, and line of reasoning whereby the reconstructed forms of the mother-tongue are arrived at.

The principles upon which our method is based are briefly stated by Brugmann (Techmer’s Zeitschrift, Bd. I., pp. 254, 255). They may be summarized as follows:

The probability that any given feature in a language is primitive increases with the number of languages in which it can be traced.

The greater the geographical separation of those languages in which the same feature occurs, the greater the likelihood that it is inherited from the mother-tongue.

Geographical separation limits the probability that the occurrence of the same peculiarity in several languages is due to contact between them at a late period, or to borrowing.

In cases where we find diversity of form in the derived languages, we assume diversity in the mother-tongue, unless we are able to show that this diversity is due to special conditions in individual languages—that is, to particular laws of sound change which we can state definitely.

It is desirable to take as wide a survey as possible, and to check the results and conclusions at which we arrive, from several sides.

In all reconstruction we must be guided by common-sense; we must bear in mind that we are dealing with sounds, and not with symbols, and must not overstep the limits of what is reasonable and probable in the sphere of actual change of sound.


CHAPTER IX

THE ARYAN OR INDO-GERMANIC MOTHER-TONGUE, AND THE DERIVED FAMILIES OF LANGUAGES

Since even the most elementary books on the History of English contain at least some statement to the effect that there once existed a language, long since extinct, which is now known as the Aryan mother-tongue, from which various groups or families of languages sprang, together with an enumeration of these, a very brief account of the present views on this subject will suffice in this place. All that need be attempted here is a short and, if possible, a clear account of what is meant by the phrase mother-tongue, an enumeration of the principal groups of languages into which this was differentiated, the supposed relationship in which they stand to each other, with a more particular account of one group—the Germanic, of which our own language is a member.

Among the numerous general authorities on the questions with which we are about to deal, there may be mentioned: Isaac Taylor, The Origin of the Aryans, 1890; Sweet, History of Language, 1900; Schrader, Sprachvergleichung und Urgeschichte, 1890; and, above all, Brugmann, Grundriss der Vergleichenden Grammatik der Indogermanischen Sprachen [2nd ed.], Bd. I. (Lautlehre), 1897; and Kurze Vergleichende Grammatik der Indogermanischen Sprachen, Bd. I. (Lautlehre), 1902, by the same author. The introductory chapters of the last two works deal with the classification and other general problems connected with the Aryan languages. The larger book should be constantly consulted by advanced students of Comparative Philology, while even beginners might with advantage consult the smaller. Brugmann’s works are standard text-books of the best kind; they are masterpieces of method, and display the latest results of modern research, more especially in so far as it deals with such problems as are settled and no longer under discussion. Brugmann represents the solid, safe, conservative wing of the new science of language, of which, together with Osthoff, Paul, Sievers, and one or two more, he was the founder more than thirty years ago. Students of the history of the Science of Comparative Philology will recognise Scherer and Leskien as the intellectual fathers of the band of scholars of whom Osthoff and Brugmann are now the distinguished and venerated chiefs.

The Conception of a Family of Languages.

The resemblances and agreements in the forms of words in vocabulary, and in inflections, which exist between such languages as Mod. Eng., Dutch, Danish, and German, are so striking that they cannot fail to impress even the least instructed student of two or more of the above languages. The farther back we go in the history of these tongues, and the earlier the forms of them which we compare, the closer becomes the resemblance. That there is an intimate connection between them is obvious. They are commonly classed together under the general name of MODERN GERMANIC LANGUAGES COMPARED the Germanic or Teutonic languages. We may take a few points of resemblance for consideration: (1) The modern Continental languages of the so-called Germanic group have, in a large number of cases, practically the same group of sounds associated with the same meaning. German kommen, ‘come,’ Dutch komme(n), Swedish komma, German tag, ‘day,’ Dutch dag (dāh), Danish dag (dæȝ); German ein, zwei, drei, vier, fünf, Dutch een, twee, drie, vier, vijf, Swedish en, twå, tre, fyra, fem = 1, 2, 3, 4, 5; German mutter, Dutch moeder, Swedish moder, ‘mother.’ And so on throughout the vocabulary, we find that these languages have in common thousands of words identical in meaning, and differing but little in pronunciation. The resemblances of Mod. Eng. to the other languages are in many cases not so close, but none the less unmistakable. (2) We find that all of these languages agree in possessing a class of so-called weak verbs, which form their past tense by the addition of the suffix -de, -te, -ed, or -ede, to the root of the verb. Eng. hear, hear-d; Swedish höra, hör-de; Dutch hooren, hoor-de; German hören, hör-te, and so on. (3) These languages all possess groups of so-called strong verbs, which form their past tenses and past participles by series of changes in the vowels of the ‘root’: Eng. sing, sang, sung; Danish synge, sang, sunget; Dutch zingen, zong, ge-zongen; German singen, sang, ge-sungen, etc.

Now, agreement between languages which includes sounds, vocabulary, inflection, and such deep-rooted features as vowel change within the ‘root’ itself, cannot be mere coincidence. Neither, when we find such common features equally among widely-separated groups of speakers, such as the Germans, Swedes, Danes, and English, can the agreement be the result of wholesale borrowing; for in this case it would naturally be asked, from whom have all these languages borrowed their characteristic features? Again, there is no reason for assuming that any one of these languages is the surviving ancestor of all the others.

There remains only the possibility that English, Dutch, the Scandinavian languages, and German, are each and all the descendants of the same original language; that they represent, in fact, the various forms into which a parent language, which no longer exists, has been differentiated, by virtue of such factors of isolation as those we have already discussed. Cf. p. 96 etc. This extinct form of speech, out of which we assume all these languages to have developed, along more or less different lines, we call Primitive Germanic, Parent Germanic, or simply Germanic. If we wished to compare the Germanic languages systematically, we should take the oldest forms of each which are preserved in writing. The above examples are drawn from the modern languages, partly because these are, on the whole, more familiar and accessible to the general student, partly also to show how close the resemblance still is, even after all these centuries of separation. The oldest considerable body of ancient Germanic speech is the fourth-century translation of part of the Bible in Gothic, a language long extinct.

By applying to the other ancient and modern languages or dialects of Europe and India tests similar to those briefly suggested above, similar results are obtained by scholars—namely, that at various points languages resolve themselves into groups of closely-related forms of speech. For each of these groups it appears necessary to CHIEF DIVISIONS OF ARYAN SPEECH assume a primitive ancestral form which no longer survives, and from which the various members of the group have been differentiated, in the same way as the Germanic languages sprang from parent Germanic.

Thus we are able, from this point of view, to distinguish the following groups or Families of Speech: (1) Indian, of which the best-known ancient representative is Sanscrit, Iranian, which includes Old (and Mod.) Persian (West Iranian), and Zend, the dialect in which the Avesta—that is, the collection of the ancient sacred books of the Parsees—is written (East Iranian). This dialect is also known as Old Bactrian. Indian and Iranian dialects are usually grouped under the general head of Indo-Iranian. The earliest remains of Sanscrit are the hymns of the Rig-Veda, the language of which is approximately 4,000 years old. (2) Armenian, whose written records go back to the fifth century of our era. (3) Hellenic, or Greek dialects. (4) Albanian, now recognised as a member of an independent group. (5) Italic, which consists on the one hand of Latin, and on the other of the Oscan and Umbrian dialects. (6) Celtic, of which ancient Gaulish was a member, but which is best known from Old and Modern Irish and Scotch Gaelic on the one hand, and from Welsh in all its stages on the other. (7) Germanic. (8) Baltic-Slavonic. The last represents two nearly-related divisions of one original group. The Baltic division is known to us from Lettish (still spoken), Old Prussian (which died out in the seventeenth century), and by Lithuanian, spoken at the present day by something between one million and a half and two million persons in Russia and East Prussia. Lithuanian records go no further back than the tenth century. The Slavonic division consists of Russian, Bulgarian, Servian (Eastern), Bohemian or Chekh (tʃɛh), Sorbian, and Polish (Western). The oldest form of Slavonic known is preserved in a translation of the Bible and other religious writings from the ninth century. The dialect is known as Old Bulgarian, Old Church Slavonic, or simply Old Slavonic.

The Aryan Family of Languages.

A comparison of the common characteristics of each of the above families of languages with the others reveals the fact that there are many features shared by the whole group of families. These consist of fundamental elements of vocabulary, such as the numerals, the substantive verb, the pronouns, the names for the natural relationships. Further innumerable suffixes and formative elements appear, under varying forms, it is true, in all the above families. They all show the same principle of vowel gradation, or differentiation of vowels in the same root, and the main outlines of sentence-structure and syntax are common to all.

Here, again, the points of agreement are too numerous and too deeply seated to be fortuitous; and the same inference is drawn with regard to the mutual relations of the various families, as were drawn from facts of the same order, in connection with the relationship of the different languages which go to make up a given family.

The assumption is made, that each of the now separate families of languages is sprung from a common parent language, the characteristics of which are preserved with varying degrees of fidelity in the derived languages. This common parent, the undifferentiated ancestral form of speech, from which it is assumed that Indo-Iranian and ‘CRADLE OF THE ARYANS’ Slavonic, and Greek and Latin, and Celtic and Germanic, have all been developed, is known as the Aryan Mother-Tongue, Primitive Aryan, or Indo-Germanic (Idg.), etc. This form of speech is, of course, nowhere spoken at the present time, nor has it ever been within the historic period. Authorities differ as to the length of time which has elapsed since the differentiation of the mother-tongue into dialects, but we may take it at something between ten and twelve thousand years.

Where was Primitive Aryan spoken?

The answer to this question, down to twenty-five years ago, was generally given in the words which the late Mr. Max Müller used, in dealing with the subject, to the end of his life—‘somewhere in Asia.’ With the exception, however, of Mr. Max Müller, and the distinguished Berlin Professor, Johann Schmidt, who died two or three years ago, probably no other responsible authority would have given such an answer—at least, not in a dogmatic manner—any time during the last quarter of a century. The question is discussed at length in the works mentioned above by Taylor, Schrader, and Sweet; and among recent contributions to the subject, the reader may also refer to Schrader, Reallexikon der Indogerm. Altertumskunde, 1901, under heading, ‘Urheimat der Indogermanen’; Hirt, Indogerm. Forsch., i., p. 464; and Kretschmer, Einl. in die Gesch. d. griech. Spr., 1896. It is sufficient here to say that the universal view now held by scholars is that the ‘Home of the undivided Aryans’ was ‘somewhere’ in Northern or Central Europe. In favour of the old view no serious argument ever has been, or ever could be, advanced, while all the evidence derived from archæology, ethnology, and comparative philology, makes for the probability of the ‘European hypothesis.’

It is to be deplored that the writers of elementary text-books, or ‘cram-books,’ as they too often are, should still continue to copy, out of the works of an earlier generation, among other views now obsolete, this particular view of migration in successive waves from Asia, which often appears in modern books of the class alluded to, not as a tentative and possible account of what happened, but in the form of a categorical statement of undisputed fact. Unfortunately, the theory has been discredited for more than thirty years.

The Aryan Race.

It used formerly to be assumed that, since affinity of language had been proved between Indians, Slavs, Germans, Greeks, Italians, and Celts, it therefore also followed that ‘the same blood flowed in the veins’ of all. At the present time probably no impartial observer would suggest such a view. The Aryan languages are obviously spoken at the present day by men of very different physical types, and certainly of distinct race. Which of the existing races who speak Aryan languages represents the original race? Perhaps none. On the other hand, it is maintained by many writers that the blonde, long-headed races of Northern Europe are nearest in physical type to the original Aryans. This question, however interesting in itself from many points of view, has but little bearing upon the problems of speech development with which we are here concerned. Whether the original speakers of Primitive Aryan were RACE AND LANGUAGE NOT COEXTENSIVE fair, like some Swedes and Russians; or dark, like other Slavs, and like some of the speakers of Irish and Welsh at the present day; or whether the mother-tongue was spoken both by fair and dark races, does not primarily concern us. We are content to know that there was a mother-tongue, which, in the course of time, spread over an immense geographical area, and was acquired by people of various racial types, who lost their own language in consequence; a fact which was probably of significance in determining the particular line of deviation from the original form, which Aryan speech followed in different areas (see ante, pp. 86 and 87).

The Relative Primitiveness of the Divisions of Aryan Speech.

As regards the preservation of inflections in their original fulness and variety, the general principle seems to be that those languages which longest preserved their old ‘free’ accent of the mother-tongue, such as Sanscrit, Greek, Baltic-Slavonic, retained also for a long time a large proportion of the original suffixes and formative elements following the root; those, on the other hand, which, like Latin, Celtic, and Germanic, developed a fixed and stereotyped accent at a comparatively early period, suffered a greater loss of inflections through the weakening of that part of words which was habitually unaccented.

When we come to consider sound changes, however, no special claim to superior general fidelity to the original quality of the sounds, in other than final syllables, can be advanced in favour of any particular group of languages. A sound is here subject to numerous changes, both Combinative and Isolative; there it appears to enjoy immunity from change. Thus, for instance, ancient Greek has preserved the rich and varied vowel system of Primitive Aryan with remarkable fidelity, but the old consonantal system undergoes many striking changes in this language: s, except when final, becomes h, and is often lost; the old back consonants with lip modification become, according to the conditions in which they appear, pure lip stops, or pure point-teeth stops; the old voiced aspirates are all unvoiced; if two aspirates of any kind follow each other in successive syllables of the same word, the first loses its aspiration. This last change is known as ‘Grassman’s Law,’ and applies also to Sanscrit. All final consonants are lost, and t before i becomes s. Sanscrit has a poor and monotonous vowel system compared with Greek; but the consonants, with the exception of the back series (back, back-outer, and back-lip-modified), are on the whole primitive. The outer varieties of back consonants become š (ʃ) and ž respectively. Latin preserves in many cases the simple vowels intact, but they are liable to various combinative changes; the diphthongs oi, eu, ou, are all levelled under ū (though O. Lat. still has oe for the first); ai becomes ae (ae), and then ē; ei becomes ī. Latin preserves faithfully the lip-modified back consonants which Greek changes so completely; but gets rid altogether of aspirated stops, which become under various conditions b, d, and f. Germanic preserves the old vowel system fairly well, but levels ā under ō, o under a, ei under ī, and oi under ai. All the stop consonants undergo change; the voiced stops are unvoiced, the voiceless stops are opened in the corresponding areas of articulation; the SCHLEICHER’S GENEALOGICAL CLASSIFICATION voiced aspirated stops also become the corresponding voiced open consonants.

Such are a few of the principal characteristic changes which take place in four important families of the Aryan languages. Clearly the paths of development are very various.

The Mutual Relations of the Chief Groups of Aryan Speech.

The problem of how to group the Aryan languages, or families of languages, among themselves in such a way as to express the degree of relationship in which they stand to each other has occupied a number of eminent scholars. Schleicher (Deutsche Sprache², p. 29) remarks, in somewhat general terms, that when two or more members of a family of languages resemble each other closely, we naturally assume that they have not been so long separated from each other, as have other members of the same family which have already diverged from each other much farther. On the grounds of this principle, and guided by what he assumed to be decisive points of resemblance, Schleicher formulated his famous ‘Stammbaum’ or genealogical tree, which expresses his conception of the interrelations of the Idg. languages and the relative periods at which they differentiated from the mother-tongue and from each other (see Compendium², 1866, p. 9). He conceives that Idg. first split into two branches (‘durch ungleiche entwickelung’)—that is to say that the ancestral form of Slavonic and Germanic (‘Slavo-deutsch’) deviated from the remaining Ursprache. Then this remaining stem, which Schleicher calls ‘Ariograekoitaloceltisch,’ divided into Arian (that is, the Indian group) on the one hand, and a dialect from which was subsequently differentiated Greek, Italic, and Celtic, on the other.

This Stammbaum theory was ruthlessly attacked by Johann Schmidt in 1872 (Verwandtschaftsverhältnisse der Idg. Spr.), who altogether rejects the old explanation of the Idg. differentiation, and substitutes for it what is known as the ‘Wellen-, or Übergangstheorie’—that is, the theory of gradual transition. Schmidt’s investigation embraced at once all the various points of agreement which exist among all the groups of Idg. speech. As a result, he believed himself justified in giving the following account of the process of the breaking up of the primitive speech. Indo-Germanic speech extended over a geographically unbroken area, in which arose from the earliest times, at different points, slight beginnings of incipient dialects in the shape of sound variation, which extended more or less far from their starting-place into the neighbouring districts. These differences grew up gradually among the speakers of what was once a homogeneous speech, and formed the prototypes of the subsequent families of languages. These dialects, however, Schmidt regarded as, in the first place, forming a continuous series, and shading one into the other. Then, here and there, the speech of one area gained in importance and strength, and absorbed those on either side which differed only slightly from it, thus destroying several links in the chain and leaving a gulf. This process happened in various centres, with the result that speech-islands were left, which differed widely from the surrounding forms. This was the origin of the great families of Idg. speech. (For good account of Schmidt’s SCHMIDT’S WELLENTHEORIE theory cf. Schrader, Sprvgl., p. 89, etc.; and Brugmann in Techmer’s Ztschr., i., p. 226, etc.)

This explanation entirely swept away Schleicher’s original ‘speech unities’ of ‘Slavo-Germanic,’ ‘Graeko-Italo-Cetic,’ etc. Schmidt showed that if the Slavonic languages could not be widely separated from the Germanic, on account of certain resemblances, too strong and too numerous to be due to coincidence, neither could the Slavonic languages be separated from the Indo-Iranian group. Greek, on the other hand, had undoubtedly close affinities to Sanscrit; but also other, equally strongly-marked characters in common with Latin. Thus the old division of the European and Asiatic branches, supposed to represent two main dialects of the Mother-Tongue, was done away with. The Gmc. family in Schmidt’s scheme comes between Slavonic and Celtic, and the latter forms the connecting-link between Gmc. and Latin, thus completing the circle of affinities. This ingenious view of gradual transitions, and the subsequent dying out of intermediate varieties, was accepted by Schrader (loc. cit.) and by Paul (in the Chapter ‘Sprachspaltung,’ Principien d. Sprgesch.).

Modifications of the ‘Übergangstheorie.’

In 1876 Leskien published his Deklination im Slavisch-Litauischen und Germanischen, in the Introduction to which he discusses the question of Idg. classification at some length. On p. x of the Introduction he criticises Schmidt’s statement of his case, and contrasts the new views with the Stammbaumtheorie. He points out that the ‘Übergangstheorie’ by itself, involves the gradual spread of population, by mere increase, over a slowly but ever increasing area. Schleicher’s explanation involves migrations of considerable magnitude, a process which would accomplish the work of differentiation far quicker and more completely. Leskien, however, does not by any means reject Schmidt’s hypothesis, but proposes to modify it, and to combine it with the theory of genealogical development. It is possible for a large community, whose speech had already two slight dialectal varieties, to migrate from their original seat and settle down, still as one community, for a long time. In this case we assume three sections, as it were, of Schmidt’s community—A, B, C, of which B’s speech forms the connecting-link between A and B, and his different points of agreement with both. Thus in their original seat A and B have had, as it were, a common speech life, so have B and C, but not A and C. Then B and C move off together, and in their new home continue their common life. Any developments subsequently undergone by A must be quite distinct from B; and, on the other hand, B may develop on lines common to C, but in which obviously A can have no share. Leskien applies this argument to the relations of Indo-Iranian, Slav.-Lith., and Gmc., and considers the treatment of Aryan ḱ and of bh-m; for this latter example I propose to substitute that of bh = Gk. φ, Gmc. and Slav. b. Indo-Iranian shares with the Baltic-Slavic languages the change of one of the original k sounds to š (ʃ), but Gmc. shows no such tendency; on the other hand, Indo-Iranian (originally, at any rate) preserves the old aspirate bh, while both Gmc. and Slav. get rid of the aspiration.

With this modification, then, Leskien’s diagram (Einleitung, LESKIEN—BRUGMANN ON ARYAN AFFINITIES p. xi) may be reproduced as follows:

A.B.C.[E]
Arian.Lith.-Slav.Gmc.
∕     ∖
ḱ < š(ʃ), s.bh < b.

The similarity between Slav.-Lith. and Gmc. in their treatment of original bh consisted primarily in the loss of aspiration; since although, later on, the individual Gmc. languages developed a voiced lip-stop (b) under certain conditions, there is reason to believe that this sound did not exist in Gmc. itself, and that bh became at first a lip-open-voice consonant.

Recent Views.

If we accept Hirt’s view of the importance of foreign influence in differentiating language, (cf. p. 85) it would seem that some such modification of Schmidt’s theory as that proposed by Leskien is necessary; since, on the one hand, it accounts for the points of resemblance between different families of Idg. speech, and, on the other, allows also for the possibility of contact with speakers of non-Idg. languages, which may explain the great diversity which also exists. With regard, however, to the features which several languages have in common, but which others do not possess, on the basis of which Schmidt postulated his system of continuous contact, Brugmann has taken up a very sceptical attitude. In an elaborate article in Techmer’s Zeitschrift für allgemeine Sprachwissenchaft, i., p. 226, etc. (Zur Frage nach den Verwandtschaftsverhältnissen der Idg. Spr.), after discussing one after another, all the special points of development which two or more groups of Idg. speech have in common, he comes to the conclusion that the majority of them prove nothing in support of the assumption of the peculiarly close relationship claimed between those groups of languages in which they occur (loc. cit., pp. 252-254). The only exception to this destructive conclusion admitted by Brugmann is the close relationship of Celtic and Italic (p. 253). The same views are maintained in the most recent pronouncements of the same author (cf. Grundriss², i., pp. 22-27; and Kurze-vergleichende Gr., pp. 3, 4, 18-22). The agreements which exist then, as they unquestionably do, between two or more speech groups, are not necessarily to be explained by assuming with Schleicher a common ‘Slavo-Germanic’ language, or a common ‘Graeko-Italic’ period.

Brugmann suggests possibilities other than the genealogical theory. The ancestors of two or more groups may have lived side by side, in a remote prehistoric period, before the breaking up of the mother-tongue, and may have developed the same tendencies in common. In such a case we should have to deal with dialectal variation originating within Aryan itself. It matters little whether, in their subsequent life-history, the languages remain in geographical contact, or become widely separated; for in the race-migrations of ages, original contiguity may be broken and joined again more than once. In grouping the languages of the Aryan stock, Brugmann arranges the families in the order suggested by their mutual resemblances; this is the most practical method of arrangement so long as it is remembered that nothing beyond resemblance is implied thereby, and that the question of how to interpret the ARYAN CONSONANTS resemblance is left open. It is possible that examples of original dialectal character are afforded by the treatment of (forward k), which becomes s or (ʃ) in Indo-Iranian and in Baltic-Slavonic, but which in all the other families is levelled under the full-back stop.

The Sounds of the Mother-Tongue.

By applying methods similar to those illustrated in the last chapter, the following sounds are now believed to have existed in Primitive Aryan:

Consonants.

Back.Back-lip-Modified.Back-outer.Front.
Openj
Stopk, kh, g, ghkʷ gʷḱ, ḱh, ǵ, ǵh
Nasalŋ
Divided
    
Blade.Point-teeth.Lip.Lip-back-Modified.
Opens, zw
Stopt, th, d, dhp, ph, b, bh
Nasalnm
Dividedl
Trillr

Vowels.[F]

Unrounded.Rounded.
Front.Back.Flat.Back.
Highī̆ū̆
Midē̆ā̆əō̆
Lowɔ̆ (?)

Also syllabic l̥, r̥, n̥, m̥; and the diphthongs: ē̆i, ē̆u, ā̆i, ā̆u, ō̆i, ō̆u.


Transcriber’s note: it was very difficult to distinguish the combination of diacritic marks macron + caron (ō̌) from macron + breve (ō̆). Here the latter is used.

The Relations of Vowels to each other in Aryan—Ablaut, or Vowel Gradation.

Cf. Brugmann; Grundr.² i., p. 482, etc., and Vgl. Gr. p. 138, etc.; Hirt d. Idg. Ablaut, 1900, and Griech. Gr., ch. ix. and x.; Streitberg Urgerm. Gr., p. 36, etc.; Noreen Urgerm. Lautlehre, p. 37, etc.; and the references given in these works.

In all Idg. languages, certain vowel changes occur within groups of etymologically related words, both in ‘roots’ and in suffixese.g.: in Gk. λέγω, ‘I speak’; λόγος, ‘word’; φᾶμί, ‘I speak’ (Doric), φωνή, ‘voice’; πατήρ, ‘father,’ Acc. πατέρα; φεύγω, ‘I fly,’ Aorist ἔφυγον, etc. In Latin, tego, ‘cover,’ perf. tēxi; moneo, literally ‘cause to remember,’ me-min-i, = *men-; dāre, ‘give’; dōnum, ‘gift’; dătus, ‘given,’ etc. In Gmc., vowel changes of this nature take place regularly in the strong verbs—e.g.: Gothic, giban, ‘give,’ pret. sing. gaf, pret. pl. gēbum, kiusan, ‘choose,’ pret. sing. kaus, pret. pl. kusum, etc.; also in other etymologically related words: O.E., dœg, ‘day,’ GRADATION A PHONOLOGICAL PROBLEM dōgor; Goth., hinþan, ‘catch,’ handus, ‘hand’ (literally, ‘that which seizes’), etc.

The above changes cannot be explained by sound laws peculiar to the particular languages in which they occur; their explanation must be sought in the common mother-tongue. The phenomena of these primitive vowel alternations are all included under the name Ablaut, invented by Grimm, although they are of various nature, and the causes which produced them must have been of several kinds; according to the present view however, it is probable that they were in all cases associated with primitive conditions of accentuation. Although the differentiation of vowels by Ablaut was made use of in Idg. to express differences of meaning, these latter are only indirectly related to the vowel changes. If a vowel originally recurred in a particular form in a particular grammatical category—as, for instance, in the Germanic strong verbs—this was because the phonetic conditions were present upon which that form of the vowel depended. The origin of Ablaut distinctions, then, is a phonological problem. Even in Idg. itself there must have been cases like that of the suffix in Gk. ῥη-τήρ, compared with ῥή-τωρ, in which the variation of the vowel performed no semasiological function at all.

The full explanation of this difficult question will probably always remain hidden, since we are here dealing with a portion of the earliest history of the Ursprache itself.

No single sound law produced all the phenomena with which the historical period of Idg. speech presents us in this respect, but a considerable number of laws, which were active at different periods, possibly widely separated in time.

The Ablaut as we know it in the earliest historic period is the result of the stratifications of the speech of different ages.

We have to distinguish two fundamentally distinct kinds of Ablaut: a Quantitative and a Qualitative. The latter kind consists in the interchange, within cognate ‘roots’ and suffixes, of vowels of different Qualitye.g., ē̆-ō̆ (cf. ῥητήρ-ῥήτωρ). The causes of this Ablaut are the most obscure.

Quantitative Ablaut, on the other hand, consists in the shortening or lengthening of vowels. This kind of Ablaut is associated mainly with the position of the accent in Primitive Aryan. By accent here may in all probability be understood stress.

It should be remembered that Idg. consisted, not of ‘Roots,’ but of words. ‘Roots’ which are mere grammatical abstractions, had no existence in Idg. any more than in Modern English. Since, however, it is necessary to make some kind of abstraction in dealing with groups of cognate words, it is better to call these ‘Bases.’ Aryan words were monosyllabic and polysyllabic, and so we speak also of monosyllabic and polysyllabic Bases.

The accent in Aryan was ‘free’—that is, the chief accent might rest, theoretically, upon any syllable in a word. In a word of several syllables only one syllable can have full stress; the other syllables have varying degrees of stress. It is enough to distinguish, from this point of view, Strong, Medium, and Weak syllables, all of these being, however, relative terms—Strong implying the chief stress in any given word, Weak implying the least stress, or what is also called absence of stress (cf. pp. 45 and 46 above).

Now, at a certain period in primitive Idg. vowels were ALTERATIONS OF VOWEL QUALITY very sensitive to the influence of stress. According to the degree of strength with which any syllable was uttered, so its original vowel or diphthong was either preserved in its full volume, or was weakened or ‘reduced.’ If the syllable was altogether unstressed, it might lose its vowel completely. The only vowels which, after the period of this weakening in unaccented syllables, could stand in strong syllables were ā̆, ē̆, ō̆, and diphthongal combinations of these with , , , , , .

We distinguish, then, three main ‘grades’ or ‘stufen’ of vowels, one of which every syllable of an Aryan word must necessarily contain: the Full grade in strong syllables, the Reduced grade in Medium syllables, and the ‘Vanishing’ grade in Weak syllables.

The ‘Dehnstufe’ or Lengthened Grade.

So far we have only considered the weakening or total disappearance of a vowel; there remains to be dealt with the further case in which an original short vowel is lengthened. To this grade German writers give the name of Dehnstufe or ‘stretch grade.’

It does not follow that all long vowels in Idg. are of this origin; there are original long vowels, which were long before the beginning of the Ablaut processes. But in word series (Ablautsreihen) in which we find long vowels side by side with short vowels, the short vowels occurring, not in the Reduced grades, but in Full grades, showing that they are original, then, in these cases, we may assume that we are in the presence of the ‘Stretch’ grade.

Compare, for instance, Latin vĕho with perf. vēxi (Idg. e-ē); O.E. sœ̆t, pret. sing. of sittan (= Idg. *sod), with sōt, ‘soot’—literally, ‘that which settles down’ (= Idg. *sōd). The explanation of this lengthening has been formulated by Streitberg (I. F., iii. 305, etc.), and has gained fairly general acceptance. Briefly stated, his law runs: ‘The short vowel of an accented (Strong) syllable is lengthened in Idg. when a following syllable is lost (cf. also Brugmann, Vgl. Gr., p. 38, and Hirt, Idg. Ablaut, p. 22, etc.). This, of course, is merely the general explanation of the origin of the lengthening in Idg. itself; it does not follow that we are always able to trace the loss of a syllable in all cases where the Dehnstufe occurs in the derived languages.

The Vowels of the Weakened Grades.

The fate of the Aryan full vowels when weakened under the conditions described above (p. 185) is clearly a matter of hypothesis. It is, however, our business to endeavour to form some idea of what happened by a comparison of all the derived languages. The reduced forms of ā, ē, ō appear in Indo-Iranian as i, and in all the other families of Aryan speech as a. It is therefore assumed that the original sound was an ‘obscure’ vowel, which is written ə in philological works.

Note.—Thus Brugmann, Grundriss,² loc. cit., and Vgl. Gr., § 127; Hirt, on the other hand (Idg. Ablaut, p. 5, etc.) assumes that these vowels did not lose their original quality in Idg. when reduced, but were merely unvoiced, and, instead of ə, writes ẹ ạ ọ. Hirt’s reason for so doing is that in Greek θετός compared with τίθημι, στατός compared with ιστᾶμι, δοτός compared with δίδωμι, the original quality of e, a, o reappears. He argues that the REDUCTION OF LONG AND SHORT VOWELS whispered vowel has emerged in Greek with mere shortening, while the other languages have lost the original quality of and , and levelled them under a. This view is also shared by Fick, Bechtel, Wackernagel, and Collitz (see references in Hirt). Brugmann, however, and probably most other scholars, explain the above Greek forms as new formations from θατός, etc.

The reduction of short a, e, o cannot be proved, from any historical indications, to have altered these vowels at all, since the original vowels reappear intact in positions where, theoretically speaking, reduction must have taken place—that is, in weak syllables. Brugmann writes these theoretical reduced vowels a, e, o, but does not discuss their nature. Hirt, again, assumes that these were voiceless (‘tonlose’) vowels. In the derived languages this grade is indistinguishable from the full grade short vowels.


Note.—The modification by accent of the long and short vowels cannot have been synchronous. We may accept Hirt’s hypothesis concerning the reduction of the short vowels, since it appears to jump with the facts. But the long vowels certainly appear to have lost their characteristic quality altogether. If this is so, then the two processes cannot have taken place at the same time, since it is scarcely conceivable that a short vowel, when unaccented, should retain its quality more completely than a long, at a period when all vowels in weak syllables were affected. We may, perhaps, assume an early period of vowel reduction which only affected short vowels, which were either unvoiced or whispered in weak syllables, but which left long vowels unaltered. Then in a subsequent period long vowels were reduced under the same conditions, only more completely than the short vowels in the former period, since they lost their quality and became an indeterminate sound (ə). We must suppose that in this period the whispered or voiceless , , which had been produced in the former age of reduction remained without further alteration. At a later period the latter class were again fully voiced, thus being levelled under the unreduced a, e, o, while ə remained until the breaking up of Aryan into dialects, and was then levelled under a in all groups except Indo-Iranian, where it became i. Qualitative Ablaut.—Under certain conditions, which are by no means clear as yet, primitive ĕ in Full Grade syllables became , and ē in the same grade became ō. Therefore, when we have a base in which primitive ĕ or ē occur, we may also expect to find cognate forms with or ō. This underwent lengthening in the Dehnstufe.

We may summarize the foregoing statement as follows

D.D°.F.F°.R.V.
e Seriesēōeoe
o Seriesōoo
a Seriesāōaoa
ē Seriesēōə
ō Seriesōə
ā Seriesāōə

Note.D. = Dehnstufe; D°. = Dehnstufe in which ō from ē occurs; F. = Full Grade; F°. that in which o from e occurs; R. = Reduced Grade; V. = Vanishing Grade.

Diphthongal Combinations in Ablaut.

Each and all the above vowels of the F. Grade occurred TREATMENT OF DIPTHONGS IN WEAK SYLLABLES in Aryan in combination with i, u, and the vocalic consonants l, m, n, r.

The long diphthongs were levelled under the original shorts, or were monophthongized in all Idg. languages except Scrt., in which there are still traces of the long (cf. Brugmann, Grundr.,² i., p. 203, etc.).

For the -i- and -u- long diphthongs we assume a R. grade əi̯, əu̯, which appear to have been levelled already in Idg. under the F. Grade before vowels. In the V. Grade the first element entirely disappears, leaving , . In all grades and are vowels before consonants, but become consonants before following vowels.

The combinations of l, m, etc., are treated in the same manner: F. el, ol; R. əl; V. l̥, etc. The ‘liquids’ and nasals in the V. Grade are consonantal before vowels, otherwise they are syllabic. The Reduced grades əi̯, əu̯, of long diphthongs appear as ī, ū before consonants; as ai̯, au̯ before vowels.

The reduced grades of the short diphthongs ei̯, ai̯, oi̯ are either levelled under the V. grade, or, when they receive a secondary accent are lengthened to ī, ū.

Although theoretically, each vowel in every word might, under the necessary conditions, appear in every grade, it does not follow that, in the derived languages, all the original possible forms of a word, ‘root,’ or suffix survive; they are very rarely all found in any one language, and some have apparently disappeared from all languages.

Examples of Aryan Ablaut.

Idg. e Series.

F.D.V.
e∥oē∥ō
Ar. *sĕd-, ‘sit’:Idg. -sd-:
  Lat. sedēreLat. sodālisLat. sēd-imusO.E. sōt  Lat. nīdus
  Gk. ἕξομαιGoth. satGoth. sētum   glt; *nisdos
  O. Sl. sedetiO.E. sǣton  O.E. nest
  O.E. sittan
   > *set-jan
Ar. *bher-:Idg. *bhr-:
  Lat. feroLat. for-s,Goth. bērumGk. φώρ  Gk. δι-φρ-ος
  Gk. φέρω    for-tūnaO.E. bǣronLat. fūr  (chariot
  Goth. bairanGk. φορᾱ́  board for two)
  O.E. beranGoth. bar
O.E. bær
Idg. bhr̥:
  Goth. baur
  O.E. boren
  ( = Gmc. *bur-)
Ar. *ped:Idg. pd-:
  Gk. πέζαLith. padasLat. pēsGk. πῶς  Gk. επί-βδ-αι-
  Lat. pĕdemGk. ποδός> *pēds    (Doric)  = *epi-pd-
Lat. ap-pod-ixGoth. fōtus
Ar. *-ter:
  Lat. paterLat. auc-torGk. πατήρGk. φρᾱ́-τωρLat. pa-tr-is
  O.E. fæderGoth. brō-þarGk. φρᾱ́-τηρGk. φρα-τρ-ά
Goth. bro-þr-ahans

The symbol < in this book means ‘becomes,’ or ‘develops into’; > means ‘derived from.’

ABLAUT SERIES

ILLUSTRATED

Idg. o Series.

F.D.V.
o.ō.
Ar. *ŏkʷ-:
  Gk. ὄσσε = *οκι̭ε;Gk. ὄπ-ωπ-α; ὢψ
    ὄψομαι
  Lat. oculus
  
Ar. *ŏd-:
  Gk. ὀδμήGk. ὀδωδή
  Lat. odor

Idg. a Series.

F.D.V.
a.ā.
Ar. *ak-:
  Scrt. ájrasGk. ἦχε (η from )Scrt. pári-jman
  Gk. ἀγρόςLat. exāmen
  Gk. ἄγω, ἄκτωρ    (> -āg-men)
  Lat. ago, actorLat. amb-āges
  Goth. akrsO.Ir. āg
  O.E. æcer
Ar. *năse:
  O.H.G. nasaLat. nāres
  Scrt.(Instr.) nasā́Lat. nāsus

Note.—According to Hirt, the forms ἀγρός, ájras, ager, akrs, also nasa and nasā́, are R. grade (cf. Idg. Abl., §§ 761-764); but the reduced grade of the e, a, o series are indistinguishable from the F. grade in the derived languages.

Idg. ē Series.

F.R.V.
ē.?[G]
Ar. *, ‘sow’:
  Lat. sēviLat. satusScrt. s-tri, ‘wife’
  Lat. sēmen
  Goth. mana-sēþs
  
Ar. *dhē, ‘place’:
  Scrt. dadhāmiScrt. hitásScrt. da-dh-mas
  Gk. τίθημι    (h from dh)
  Lat. fēciGk. τιθεμεν
  Goth. gadēþsLat. facio
  O.E. dǣd
  
Ar. *lēd, ‘let,’ ‘grow
    tired’:
  Gk. ληδεῖνLat. lassus
  Goth. lētan    > *lad-to-
  O.E. lǣtanGoth. lats

Idg. ō Series.

F.R.V.
ō.?[G]
Ar. *do-, ‘give’:
  Scrt. dadātiScrt. a-ditasdēvá-t-tas
  Gk. δίδωμιScrt. ditiš  (-t- from -d-)
  Gk. δώσωGk. δίδομενLat. dē-d-i
  Lat. dōnumLat. datus
  Lat. dōnareLat. datio
  
Ar. *bhōg-, ‘roast’:
  Gk. φώγωGk. φαγεῖν
  O.E. bōc (pret. ofO.E. bac-an
    bacan)O.E. bæcere

POLYSYLLABIC

BASES

Idg. ā Series.

F.R.V.
ā?[G]
Ar. *sthā-, ‘stand’:
  Gk. ἵστημιScrt. sthitásScrt. gō-ṣ̌ṭh-á
  Gk. στήσωGk. ἴ-στα-μεν  (‘standing-place
    (η from ā)Gk. στατός    for cows’)
  Lat. stāreLat. status
  Lat. stāmenLat. statimGoth. awistr
  Goth. stōlsGoth. staþs  (= *ou̯i-st-tro)
    ‘sheep-fold’
O.H.G. ewist > *awist
  
Ar. *bhā, ‘speak’:
  Gk. φημí (*φᾶμí)Gk. φαμεν
  Lat. fāri
  Lat. fāma

Transcriber’s Note: this character is illegible. It might be Latin small letter barred o (ɵ) or Latin small letter turned e (ǝ).

For an account and full examples of the Ablaut in original polysyllabic bases, see Brugmann and Hirt, loc. cit., especially the latter. In dealing with these bases, it is necessary to distinguish the vowel gradation in each syllable. A few examples may be given here (the numbers refer to syllables):

Aryan *genewo, ‘knee.’

Scrt. jā́nu, Gk. γόνυ, have F. in 1st, R. in 2nd; Goth. kniu (= *ǵnewo-), O.E., cneō, have V. in 1st, F. in 2nd; Scrt. abhi-jnú, ‘down to the knee,’ Gk. γνύξ, πρόχνυ, Goth. knussjan, have V. in 1st, R. in 2nd.; while D. grade appears in Gk. γωνιά, in 1st.

Aryan *ǵenē, *ǵonē, *ǵenō, *ǵonō, ‘know.’

Goth. kann has F. (Idg. *ǵon-); Lith. žinóti, Goth. kunnaida, have R. or V. in 1st (Idg. *ǵn̥-) and F. in 2nd; Scrt. a-jña-sam, jñā-tás, Gk. γι-γνώ-σκω, Lat. nōsco, O.E. cnāwan, have V. in 1st (Idg. ǵn-) and F. in 2nd; O.H.G. kunst (Idg. *ǵn̥-t-to) has R. in 1st and V. in 2nd.

Aryan *pelē, ‘fill.’

Scrt. parīnas (r from l) has F. in 1st and 2nd; Scrt. pr̥nāti, Lat. plēnus, etc., Gk. πλῆ-ρες, etc., have V. in 1st, F. in 2nd; Scrt. pūrnás, Lith. pílnas, Goth. fulls, have R. in 1st, V. in 2nd.

Aryan *perō, *perem, ‘forward.’

Gk. πρωί, O.H.G. vruo (= *frō), have V. in 1st, F. in 2nd; Lith. pirmas, O.E. forma (= *furma > Idg. *pr̥mo-), have R. in 1st, F. in 2nd (or 3rd if we assume pre-Idg. *peremo); Goth. fruma, O.E. from (= *pr̥mo), have R. in 1st, V. in 2nd (*peremo), and F. in 3rd.

The phenomena of Ablaut are to be regarded as a series of Combinative Changes which took place in the mother-tongue. They are among the most characteristic features of Aryan speech. If primitive Aryan be a dialect of a still older language, then we may consider that its characteristic independent life as Aryan begins with the first Ablaut changes.


CHAPTER X

THE GERMANIC FAMILY

This Family, which is of special importance to students of English, falls into three divisions—the North Germanic or Scandinavian; the East Germanic, represented by Gothic and the language of the Vandals, both long extinct, and the latter only preserved in proper names; West Germanic, the earliest forms of which are Old Saxon, the Old English dialects, Old Frisian, all of which belong to the so-called Low German group, and Old High German, the name given to a group of West Germanic dialects in which the voiceless stops of Germanic, preserved in all other dialects and languages of this family, underwent a change to open consonants or affricated sounds respectively, during the sixth and seventh centuries. Other consonants also underwent change, but less universally than Gmc. p, t, k, though even in the case of k the opening or affrication was not carried out with perfect uniformity, in all positions, in every H.G. dialect. Within the West Germanic branch itself, it is now usual to assume an Anglo-Frisian group, which subsequently differentiated into Old Frisian and Old English. (For statement and arguments in favour of this view, see especially Siebs, Zur Gesch. d. engl-friesisch. Spr., 1889, and Bremer, Ethnographie der germ. Stämme² 1900, p. 108, etc. The latter is a reprint from Paul’s Grundr.², in which see p. 842, etc.) This assumption of an original Anglo-Frisian unity is based upon certain very close agreements in vocabulary, and in the treatment of the vowel sounds, which exist between O.E. and O. Fris. At the same time, the Anglo-Frisian unity, although a very plausible hypothesis, is contested by some scholars (e.g., Morsbach, Beibl. zur Anglia, vii., and Wyld, Engl. Studien, xxviii., pp. 393, 394, Otia Merseiana, iv., pp. 75, 76), and a further critical examination of the points of agreement between the two languages is desirable in order to determine how far these are really due to a common, and how far to an independent, development.

[On the classification of the Germanic languages, their mutual relations and characteristics, the best authorities are: Kluge, Vorgeschichte der germanischen Sprachen in Paul’s Grundriss²; Streitberg, Ur-germanische Grammatik, pp. 9-18 (the latter book is perhaps the best introduction to the study of Germanic Philology which exists); Einleitendes in Dieter’s Laut- und Formenlehre d. altgermanischen Dialekte, vol. i., 1898. The above works contain full references to the special grammars of the several languages, and to authorities on the various questions of general and special bearing connected with Germanic Philology.]

Primitive Germanic.

By this term is meant, as already indicated, that undifferentiated form of speech, distinguished from Primitive Aryan by possessing the characteristic Germanic features, and containing the germ of those peculiarities which subsequently appear in those languages, already enumerated, which spring from this source. The sources of our knowledge FORMS OF GERMANIC—HOW ARRIVED AT of Parent Germanic are of a twofold character: Direct and Indirect.

The direct sources of knowledge are scanty, and consist (1) of Gmc. words mostly occurring in proper names mentioned in the works of Greek and Latin writers from the time of Cæsar; and (2) very early loan-words from Gmc. still preserved in Finnish, which in many cases retain down to the present day the original full Gmc. form. The indirect sources are (1) the earliest Runic inscriptions in Primitive Norse, some of which are as old as the first century of our era, and the language of which is therefore but a stage removed from Primitive Gmc.; and (2) the reconstructions which are made according to the strict methods of modern Comparative Philology (cf. Chapter VIII.).

Characteristics of Germanic.

At what point of the original Aryan dialectal differentiation does Germanic come into existence? Can we say that when a certain group of features have developed within a speech area this ceases to be Primitive Aryan any longer, but has now an independent existence with the definitely-marked features of the ancestor of the Germanic languages?

Probably the most characteristic and typical Germanic characteristics are the consonantal changes, the so-called sound-shifting processes, known to the readers of text-books as Grimm’s Law. We might perhaps say that from the moment that original t, p, k, have become open consonants, here is the beginning of Gmc. Since none of the readers (and few of the writers) of the ordinary small primer which discourses glibly of Grimm’s Law have any idea where that Law is to be found in the works of Grimm, nor how he states it, it may be of interest to mention that in vol. i. of the Deutsche Grammatik, p. 584, etc. (I quote from the edition of 1822), the immortal grammarian discusses, with numerous examples, the relations of the consonantal sounds of Sanscrit, Greek, and Latin, etc., with those of Gothic and Old High German. Grimm also notes that in certain Gothic words ‘exceptions’ occur to the usual correspondences of Gk., Lat., Scrt. p, t, k, to Gothic f, þ, etc. These exceptions were to be explained some fifty years later by Verner.

The statement of these facts of consonantal change which would be accepted at the present day is very different from Grimm’s statement, as the reader may see by comparing the treatment of the subject by Streitberg, for example, with the above passages in Grimm’s Grammar.

The Consonantal Shiftings in Germanic.

I. Aryan p, t, k were aspirated to ph, th, kh, being thus levelled under the original voiceless aspirated stops.

II. All the voiceless aspirated stops, both old and new, were opened, and became the corresponding voiceless open CONSONANTAL CHANGES—GRIMM—VERNER consonants.

Examples:

{ph (original); O. Sax. and O.H.G. fallan, ‘fall’; Gk. σφάλλω.
Aryan{ph (from earlier p); Goth. -faþs, ‘lord,’ ‘master’; Scrt. páti-, ‘master’; Gk. πόσις (from *potis), ‘husband’; Lat. hos-pit-is (gen.), ‘guest-friend.’
  
{th (original); Goth. skaþjan, ‘to harm’; Gk. ἀ-σκηθής, ‘blameless.’
Aryan{th (from earlier t); Goth. munþs; O.E. mūþ, ‘mouth’; Lat. mentum, ‘chin.’
  
{kh (original);   ?
Aryan{kh (from earlier k); Goth. hairtō, ‘heart’; O.E. heorte; Gk. καρδἰα; Lat. cord-is (gen.).

These changes invariably take place initially; medially, however, when the accent in Aryan fell on any other syllable than that immediately preceding them, the Gmc. consonants f, þ, h (back-open cons.) were voiced to ƀ (lip-open-voice), ð (point-teeth-open-voice), and ȝ (written g in most old Germanic languages, but = back-open-voice). These were the ‘exceptions’ to his law which puzzled Grimm, but which were explained as above by Verner (Kuhn’s Zeitschrift, xxiii., pp. 97-130) in 1877. Sanscrit and Greek often preserve the original accent, so that where we find b, d, g, in Germanic, instead of the voiceless sounds, the Greek forms often show the accent on some other syllable than that immediately preceding the consonant. This habit of voicing in the Germanic languages, under the above conditions, proves that parent Germanic retained the original system of ‘free’ accent, since the same root shows voiceless or voiced forms according to the shifting position of the accent.

Examples of Verner’s Law:

Aryan p (or ph) = Gmc. ƀ (written b); Goth. and O. Sax. sibun, ‘7’; Scrt. saptá; Gk. ἑπτά.

Aryan t (th) = Gmc. đ (written d); Goth. fadar, ‘father’; O.E. fœder; Scrt. pitár; Gk. πατήρ.

Aryan k = Gmc. ȝ (written g); O.E. sweger, ‘mother-in-law’; Scrt. svašrū́; Gk. ἑκυρᾱ́, from *σϝεκυρᾱ́.


Note.—The old Germanic languages do not distinguish b, d, g, according to whether they represent open consonants or stops. Originally these consonants were all open in Gmc. It is usual for philologists, for purposes of accuracy, to write these original open consonants Ѣ, đ, ȝ. The popular expression that ‘h became g by Verner’s law’ is most mischievous, and gives a false impression. We are dealing with changes which took place hundreds of years before writing was known to the Gmc. peoples—with pure sound changes. The facts are simply and accurately stated by saying that the lip, point-teeth, and back voiceless open consonants were voiced. That is the process which took place under the conditions described by Verner.

The Third Germanic Consonant Shifting.

The Aryan aspirated voiced stops, bh, dh, gh, are opened in Gmc. to the corresponding voiced open consonants.

The Ѣ, đ, ȝ thus produced are indistinguishable from the same sounds which arose according to the conditions of Verner’s Law; they share in each language the subsequent development of these, and are also written b, d, g in the old languages.

These voiced aspirates survive, as such, only in Sanscrit; in Gk. they remain as aspirates (apart from certain combinative COMPLETION OF THE SHIFTINGS changes), but are unvoiced, and are written φ, θ, χ.

Examples:

Aryan dh, Gmc. đ: Goth. ga-dē-þ-s, ‘deed’; O.E. dǣd; Scrt. dá-dhā-mi, ‘set, place’; Gk. τί-θη-μι.

Aryan bh, Gmc. Ѣ: Goth. brōþar, ‘brother’; O.E. brōþor; Scrt. bhrā́-tar; Gk. φρᾱ́τωρ.

Aryan gh, Gmc. ȝ: Goth. steigan, ‘climb, ascend’; O.E. stigan; Scrt. stighnutē; Gk. στείχω.

The Fourth and Last Consonantal Shifting in Germanic.

The Aryan voiced stops b, d, g, were unvoiced in Gmc. to the corresponding breath-stops p, t, k.

There is an indication of the approximate date of these processes of shifting in place-names. The mountain name Finne was borrowed by the Suevi from the Gaulish penn, after they crossed the Elbe in the fifth century b.c. Therefore the change from p to f was subsequent to this. On the other hand, the Gmc. Dōnavi, ‘Danube,’ from Latin Dānuvius, preserves the d unchanged, which shows that the change from d to đ had already taken place before the incorporation of this name in Gmc. speech, which occurred about 100 b.c. (On the relative chronology of the shifting processes, see Kluge, Paul und Braune’s Beitr., ix., 173, etc., and Streitberg, loc. cit., § 126.)

Examples of Fourth Shifting of Voiced Stops:

Aryan b, Gmc. p: Goth. paida, ‘coat’; O.E. pād; Gk. (Thracian) βαίτη, ‘shepherd’s coat of skins.’

Aryan d, Gmc. t: Goth. ga-tamjan, ‘tame’; O.E. temian; Gk. δαμάω; Lat. dom-are.

Aryan g, Gmc. k: O.E. cran, ‘crane’; O. Sax. crano; Gk. γέρανος.

Characteristic Treatment of the Aryan Vowels in Germanic.

A. Isolative Changes.

Aryan o is unrounded to a in Gmc.: Lat. ovis, ‘sheep’; Gk. ὄις, from *όϝις; Goth. awis-tr, ‘sheepfold’; Lat. hostis, ‘enemy,’ ‘stranger’; Goth. gast-s; O. Sax., O.H.G. gast, ‘guest.’ Thus original o and a are indistinguishable in Gmc.

Aryan ā is rounded to ō in Gmc., and is thus levelled under original ō: Gk. φρᾱ́τωρ, ‘brother’; Lat. frāter; Goth. brōþar; O.E. brōþor; Lat. sāgire, ‘perceive quickly and keenly’; Goth. sōk-jan, ‘seek.’

Aryan ē is lowered to ǣ in Gmc. This ǣ is again raised to ē in Goth.; in West Gmc. it becomes ā, and in O.E. this ā is again fronted to ǣ: Gk. τί-θη-μι, ‘place,’ etc.; Goth. ga-dēþs, ‘deed’; O.H.G. tāt; O.E. dǣd; Gk. νῆ-μα, ‘thread’; Lat. -re, ‘sew’; Goth. nēþla, ‘needle’; O.H.G. nādala; O.E. nǣdl.

Aryan oi is levelled under ai in Gmc.: Gk. οἴνη, ‘one, upon a die’; O. Lat. oinos (later ūnus); Goth. ains; O. Lat. moitare (later mūtare), ‘change’; Goth. maidjan, ‘alter.’

Aryan ou is levelled under au in Gmc.: Gk. οὖς, from *οὔος, from *οὔσος, ‘ear’; Lat. auris, from *ausis, from *ousis; Goth. auso; Gk. ἀ-κούω, from Aryan *sm̥-kous-jō, ‘hear’; Goth. haus-jan, ‘hear.’

Aryan ei becomes ī in Gmc.: Gk. πείθω, ‘persuade’; Lat. fīdo, from *feido; Goth. beidan, ‘expect’ (ei in Goth. = ī); O.E. bīdan; O.H.G. bītan.

[Aryan ēi is probably the origin of an ē sound which appears as such in the Gmc. languages.]

The other Aryan vowels are unaffected by isolative change in Gmc.

B. Combinative Changes.

Aryan e, which is otherwise preserved in Gmc., is raised VOWEL CHANGES to i in Gmc. under the following conditions: (1) Before i or j in the following syllable: Gk. μέσσος (from *μεθ-ϳος); Lat. medius; Goth. midjis; O.E. midd; O. Sax. middi, Gk. ἓζομαι (from *σεδϳομαι), ‘sit’; Lat. sed-ēre; O. Sax. sittian; O.E. sittan (from *sett-jan); O.H.G. sizzen. (2) e becomes i when followed by a nasal + another consonant: Gk. πενθερός, ‘father-in-law’ (literally, ‘relation’); Lith. bendras, ‘companion,’ from Lat. of-fend-ix, from root *bhendh-; Goth., O.E., O. Sax. bindan.

[e also becomes i in Gmc. in unstressed syllables; cf. O.E. pl. fēt, ‘feet,’ from *fōtiz (nom. sing. fōt), Lat. ped-es.]

Apart from these conditions, e remains in Gmc.: Gk. ἔδω, ‘eat’; Lat. edo; O.E., O. Sax. etan; Gk. ἔργον, ‘work’ (from *ϝέργον); O. Sax. werk; O.H.G. werc; and so on.

West Germanic Characteristics.

The Gmc. sound system underwent but few changes in W. Gmc., but these few are important.

The change of ǣ to ā has already been mentioned. In addition, the combinative treatment of i and u must be noted.

Gmc. i remains in W. Gmc., unless followed in the next syllable by ā̆, or ō̆, in which case it was lowered to e. O.E., O.H.G. nest, ‘nest,’ from *nizdo (cf. Lat. nīdus, from *nizdos).

Of course, if n + consonant intervened between i and ā̆, ō̆, i remained. Gmc. u also remained, apart from the presence of a following ā̆, ō̆, in which case it was lowered to o in W. Gmc.: O.E. oxa; Goth. auhsa (= *uhsa); Scrt. ukṣan; O.E. gold, ‘gold,’ from Gmc. *gulđo; cf. kulta, ‘gold,’ a very early Gmc. loan-word in Finnish.

The above account of the treatment of Aryan sounds in Germanic is the merest outline. The question of the lip-modified back consonants, of consonantal combinations, and of the special W. Gmc. treatment of i and u between vowels, have not been dealt with; on all these points the reader should consult Streitberg’s Urgerm. Grammatik.


CHAPTER XI

THE HISTORY OF ENGLISH: GENERAL REMARKS ON THE SCOPE AND NATURE OF THE INQUIRY, AND THE MAIN PROBLEMS CONNECTED WITH IT

If it were necessary to answer as briefly as possible the question, What does the history of English involve? it might be said that, given the English language as it now exists, in all its forms, spoken and written, historical inquiry should attempt to trace the origin and development of the characteristic features of each.

This is the ideal of completeness; practically the history of English is mainly concerned with the rise, on the one hand, of present-day polite spoken English, and, on the other, with that of the literary dialect. The problems herein involved are sufficiently complicated, and the history of the modern dialects, or forms of popular speech, at any rate in its minute detail, is held to be the work of the special investigator. At the same time, it is important to have some conception of the popular dialects, and to understand as clearly as possible their mutual relations, as well as their relation to, and influence upon, the more cultivated and artificial forms of English speech.

Two methods of procedure are open to the student. He may either start with the language as he knows it, and trace it backwards, step by step, to the earliest forms preserved in the oldest written documents; or, starting with these, he may work forwards to the present day. Whichever method be chosen, it is necessary to have at least some knowledge of the language at each stage of its development, and, further, it is of the highest importance that the student should endeavour to realize as far as possible each stage as a living language which was actually spoken. In fact, every step we take into the past of a language involves a process of reconstruction: first, an interpretation of the written symbols, and then the gradual realization of the consciousness of the part, so that the sentences begin to pulsate with life, and become for us the living expression of the thoughts and emotions of the men who uttered them. There can be no doubt that the best way to cultivate this power of getting into sympathetic touch with the speech of a bygone age is to train the perceptions and the sensibilities in the school of modern speech, and for this reason, as well as for others repeatedly argued in these pages, the study of the spoken language of our own time is the best training-ground for historical study.

Each period of the development of English presents special problems to the investigator—problems which depend partly upon the nature of the changes which the language itself undergoes, partly upon the social conditions and general historical and political events which affected the linguistic conditions, and partly, also, upon the form in which the records of each age have come down to us. The minute investigation of the dialectal varieties in Old and Middle English is the business of the specialist, and many of the details which are of great interest and ENGLISH SPEECH IN EARLIER PERIODS importance for him have but little bearing upon the development of present-day English.

The solution of one and the same kind of problem may demand a different method at different times. Thus the reconstruction of the pronunciation, which is necessarily our first care in dealing with the written records of all periods earlier than our own, offers difficulties of quite a different kind in Old English from those which meet us in attempting to realize the sounds of Shakespeare. In the latter case we have a considerable body of direct contemporary testimony, sometimes, it is true, rather contradictory, as to the phonetic values expressed by the symbols in ordinary spelling; in the former the precise sound which the letters were intended to express can only be inferred indirectly from the spelling of foreign words of whose pronunciation at the time something is known, by the help of comparative philology, or by considering the later developments, since the O.E. period. On the other hand, in dealing with the written language of periods which had no stereotyped orthography, we have, at any rate, the advantage of being warned by a change in the spelling of a probable change in sound, whereas for the last 400 years—although, as can be shown from other sources, considerable changes in English pronunciation have taken place—the spelling during this period has varied so little that, were there no other means of information, we might suppose that sound change had been arrested since early in the sixteenth century.

Probably the best course for the student of the history of English to pursue is first to make himself acquainted with the chief characteristics of each period, and then to construct for himself as complete a picture as possible of the gradual passing of the speech of one period into that of the next, until the whole space of time covered by the records is filled in. A narrative which should thus set forth in outline the changes through which our language has passed during the last 1,200 years, might with advantage, in the first instance, be limited to the history of the modern literary language, and that form of spoken English which most closely resembles it. The question would thus be, What is the relation of these modern forms to the earlier forms of English? The scope of this inquiry might be extended, especially by Scotch students, so as to include the rise of Scots, as a form of speech so distinct from English, that it deserves to be ranked as another language. No other group of English dialects, except those out of which the literary and polite spoken English grew, possesses the distinction which Scots achieved of being for centuries the speech of kings and scholars, of poets and historians; the language at once of the Court, the Government, the Church, and of Literature.

Besides the problems connected with changes in sound, the student of the history of English must naturally trace the modifications in the inflexional system which have taken place, many of which are also associated with sound change. The impoverishment of the English grammatical inflexions has been due very largely to phonetic changes which have occurred in the unstressed syllables of words, whereby many final syllables have been lost altogether, while others have been very considerably altered from their original form. The changes in our accidence, especially the loss of many case-endings, have brought THE SOURCES OF LOAN-WORDS about very marked changes in the form and structure of the sentence.

Inseparable, too, from the growth of culture, and from a general expansion of a nation’s genius, is the development of the vocabulary. It is natural that the meaning of words should change as the group of ideas associated with a given word is now widened, now contracted, but perhaps the most considerable modifications of our vocabulary at all ages have come from without, by the incorporation of altogether new material from other languages. Every text-book upon the history of English contains more or less reliable lists of foreign words which have passed at various times, and from different sources, into usage in the English tongue. It will be convenient to deal with the question of loan-words under a separate heading within each section which is devoted to a period in the growth of English. Points of interest in connection with this subject are: to distinguish words of foreign origin which have got into English, through the spoken language, from those which have been incorporated from merely literary sources; to determine the period at which any given word or class of words passed into English. One of the chief popular fallacies in dealing with loan-words is the assumption that the latter question can be settled out of hand by an appeal to history. Thus, for instance, it is commonly assumed by popular writers that all Latin words which occur in Old English, and which refer to ideas or objects connected with the Christian religion, were incorporated into English at the time of the mission of St. Augustine. As a matter of fact, some of these words are centuries older, and were certainly acquired by the heathen English, already in their Continental homes. The one sure test of the immediate source of an early loan-word, and the date of its importation, is its form, and the consideration of the changes which it has undergone in common with the native element of the language into which it has been borrowed. If this test cannot be applied, as is sometimes the case, there always remains a certain dubiety as to the precise period of borrowing.

In studying the various forms of English preserved in the literary remains of the Old and Middle periods, it is important to keep the several dialects distinct, and, further, not to confuse the language of different ages. It often happens that a work comes down to us in several manuscripts, copied at different times by a variety of scribes, whose native dialect is not always the same as that of the original. In such cases there is naturally a mixture of dialectal forms, and not infrequently, also, a mixture of forms which belong to the period of the original with those which are contemporary with the copy. This confusion arises from the fact that the scribe sometimes faithfully copied his text, but sometimes also wrote the form which was current in his own speech, instead of the more archaic form of his model.

Therefore the study of the dialect of a given area, at a given period, must be based, in the first instance, upon texts whose date and dialect can be fixed beyond any doubt. Although the spelling in Old and Middle English texts is on the whole fairly consistent and regular, there is always the apparently exceptional spelling, which occurs here and there, and which deserves attention. The questions raised by the occasional departure of scribes INCONSISTENCIES OF SPELLING IN EARLY MSS. from the conventional spelling are: Do they represent a new tendency which is springing up within the dialect, a new departure from the older mode of speech which the traditional spelling records, and which the scribe from time to time, either deliberately or unconsciously, expresses in a phonetic spelling? Are they mere careless scribal errors? Do they represent another type of pronunciation in use within the dialect, due to class or other differentiation, or to the influence of another dialect? While it is unwise to attach too much importance to sporadic eccentricities of spelling on the part of a scribe, they should all receive consideration, and anything like repeated deviation from the tradition should be carefully investigated, since if it can be shown to express some reality of pronunciation, it is certainly of value, and may throw great light upon the speech habits of the period.

Chief Points of General Method.

There are certain general principles of method which should be constantly borne in mind in the historical study of language, and these may now be summarized, even at the risk of repetition, for they follow logically from that view of language which this work has attempted to set forth, and some of the principles have already been formulated in this and in earlier chapters.

1. We must not be misled by the inconsistency of the written representation of sounds in early records, into assuming an inconsistency of pronunciation. Such inconsistency of spelling may occur while the pronunciation itself is perfectly constant. A fluctuation in the graphic representation of sounds is particularly likely to occur in a period in which a series of sound changes are in process of being carried out, or have just been completed. The fluctuation in spelling may make it appear as though, in the same text, there were traces both of the beginning and the end of a particular process of sound change. Even when a spelling is to a great extent phonetic, as in O.E., it will generally be slightly behind the actual pronunciation.

2. Apparent anomalies in the development of sounds, or ‘exceptions’ to well-established sound laws, may result from a mixture of dialectal forms; and the ‘exception’ may prove to be merely an importation from another dialect in which that particular line of development is quite normal. The mixture of dialects is especially common in literary forms of language, which represent historically the pure form of no single dialect, but a conglomeration of several. The higher the development and cultivation of a literary dialect, the more artificial it is likely to be, and the further removed from any naturally-developed form of living speech. Good examples of artificial literary dialects are the Greek κοινή, Classical Latin, and Modern Polite English. In O.E. and early M.E. the various forms of written English each represent pretty accurately the dialect of the province in which the text was written. But Chaucer’s English is no longer the dialect of a particular geographical area, but rather a fully-developed literary or official form of speech which shows considerable dialectal mixture. These literary or official dialects often become, with certain modifications, the traditional mode of speech of a social class, or even of a whole country.

3. Many apparent ‘exceptions’ are the result of GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF HISTORICAL METHOD Analogy, and not of Phonetic development at all. The history of every language has numerous examples of forms of this nature. In Mod. Eng. the preterites of ‘break’ and ‘speak’ are not the representatives of O.E. brœc, sp(r)œc, but are formed on the analogy of the p.p. brok-en, spok-en. This process of forming new associations, as we have seen (Chapter VII.), is always at work at all periods of every language. In postulating Analogy in explanation of a form which has not followed the ordinary phonetic development, it is our business to discover the group of forms associations with which has caused the new departure in question.

4. After a sound has changed, within the dialect of a given community, to something quite different from its original form, the same sound may reappear within the same dialect from some other source, and may then remain, the tendency to change it having passed away. The Southern and Midland dialects of English rounded all O.E. ā sounds to ō (ɔ̄) in early Transition M.E., O.E. hām, etc., becoming hōm, etc. But in M.E. ā reappeared again from two sources: (1) O.E. -ă- in open syllables was lengthened—O.E. sċ(e)amu < M.E. schāme. (2) Norman-French ā in loan-words—e.g., dāme, ‘lady.’ This new ā survived during the whole M.E. period, until it was fronted in the sixteenth century to (ǣ), which later became (ē), whence Standard English (ɛi) as in ‘shame’ (ʃϵim) and ‘dame’ (dɛim).

5. Where diversity of sound exists, we assume it to represent original diversity, unless the conditions whereby one sound was differentiated into several, can be clearly shown. Thus in O.E. the vb. ‘to bear’ has the following forms of the root: Inf. ber-an, pret. sing. bœr, pret. pl. bœ̄r-on, p.p. bor-en. Here we assume that there were originally four distinct forms of the root in Gmc., since nothing that we know of the habits of O.E. leads us to believe that any conditions are present in these cases to split up one sound into four; and, further, a comparison of the other old Gmc. tongues points also to the conclusion that so far as Gmc. is concerned, there were always four distinct forms of the root (cf. examples of e-series of Aryan Ablaut, under *bher- in Chapter IX.). On the other hand, if we take the three vowels a, e, ea, in the O.E. racu, ‘narrative reċċean, inf. ‘to narrate’; reahte, pret. ‘narrated,’ we have every reason to assume that in this case one original Gmc. sound a has been differentiated into three sounds in O.E. itself, and the conditions of that differentiation can be stated (cf. Chapter XII., sections on i-mutation and Fracture). Thus we should reconstruct the earlier forms *raka-, *rœkk-jan, *rah-ta, respectively, to correspond to the three O.E. forms above.

6. The same sound, as we have just seen, may have a various development in the same dialect under different phonetic conditions. Later on, when the tendencies of combinative change which produced the variety have passed away, the different forms may be used promiscuously, and without regard to the original conditions under which they severally arose. It should be remembered that combinative change may operate not only within what we call the ‘word,’ but also within the breath-group, or, as it often is, the sentence.

The two words ‘of’ and ‘off’ in Modern English, were originally doublets of the same word, the voiced final DOUBLETS DUE TO VARYING STRESS consonant occurring in cases where the word was unstressed in the sentence, the voiceless final when it was stressed. Now the two forms are independent and distinct words, each specialized to express a different meaning; and although ‘of,’ as it happens, is usually without stress, ‘off’ may be used equally in stressed or unstressed positions. In the same way the word seint, ‘saint,’ had two forms in M.E.: (sin) in unstressed positions, (saint) when stressed. The latter strong form has become Mod. Eng. ‘saint’ (sɛint); the former has become (sən or sənt), as in St. Andrews (sənt ændrūz) or St. John, the name of the Apostle (əen džɔn). But in the family name St. John, pronounced (sindžən), the stress has been shifted to the first syllable, which, however, still preserves the original form which it acquired in unstressed positions; and the same is true of the name St. Leger (silidžə) as regards the vowel, although here the -n has been lost. The substantive ‘saint,’ however, always preserves the strong or stressed form, even when it occurs with weak stress in a sentence.

The principles of modern philological method have been formulated on various occasions, notably by Brugmann—e.g., Morphol. Untersuch., i., p. xiii, etc.; Zum heutigen Stand der Sprachwissensch., p. 53, etc.; Grundr.², pp. 63-72; Griech. Gr.³, pp. 2-9.


CHAPTER XII

HISTORY OF ENGLISH: THE OLD ENGLISH PERIOD

The designation Old English is applied to that period of the history of our people which extends from the first settlement of Germanic tribes in these islands down to the coming of the Normans. The O.E. period of the language may roughly be estimated as reaching down to 1050, after which period the chief features of the next, or Transition period from Old to Middle English, begin to be fairly well established, and expressed in the written forms which have come down to us.

Within the O.E. period of the history of the language it is possible to distinguish, from the documents, three stages of development, which are known respectively as the Earliest, down to 750; Early, down to 900; Late, down to 1050. The dates here given are, of course, only approximate, since neither the imperfection of the series of records, nor the slow and gradual mode of growth in language, permit us to make a precise hard-and-fast division between different periods.

There are three chief types of dialectal variety distinguishable from the records: Saxon, of which West Saxon became the principal dialect of literature; Kentish, the dialect of the Jutes; Anglian, which includes both Northumbrian CLASSIFICATION OF DOCUMENTS and Mercian.

Sources of our Knowledge of O.E.

Practically everything of value from a literary point of view is preserved in W.S., having been either written in that dialect originally or copied into it at a later period. There are a certain number of Charters, which possess great historical interest, in other dialects, especially Kentish. There is little original prose, except Homilies and Laws, which are mainly W.S. in form; and of the translated literature the greatest part, and that which is of the chiefest interest, the authentic works of King Alfred, is in the same dialect—the other dialects, apart from charters, being represented almost entirely by translations of the Psalms and interlinear versions of the New Testament. There are glossaries, which are of great value to students of the language, in Saxon, Kentish, and Mercian dialects. The poetical literature, with the exception of a few fragments in Early Northumbrian, exists in manuscripts of the tenth and eleventh centuries in a dialect which, while it is largely W.S., yet shows numerous characteristics of other dialects, the result, probably, of late copying from Anglian by W.S. scribes.

The following is a list of the chief remains which are important for the study of the several dialects. It will be noticed that very little Earliest W.S. has been preserved.

A. Earliest Texts.

1. Northumbrian.Northumbrian Fragments, in Sweet’s Oldest English Texts, p. 149, etc. Liber Vitæ, O.E.T., p. 153, etc. Northumbrian Genealogies, O.E.T., p. 167, etc. Names in Moore MS. of Bede’s Eccl. Hist., O.E.T., p. 131, etc.

2. Mercian.Epinal Glossary (circa 700), Corpus Glossary (circa 750), in O.E.T. Charters of eighth century (Latin, containing Eng. words and names), O.E.T., p. 429, etc.

3. Kentish.Charters (Latin, but containing Eng. words and names), O.E.T., p. 427, etc. These documents belong to seventh and eighth centuries; the earliest of these, No. 4 in O.E.T., is the oldest written document we possess containing English forms.

4. West Saxon.Charter No. 3 in O.E.T.

B. Ninth-Century Texts (Early).

1. Northumbrian.

2. Mercian.Vespasian Psalter and Hymns, O.E.T., p. 183, etc.; the Hymns also Sweet, A.S. Reader, p. 117, etc.

3. Kentish.Numerous Charters, mostly English, O.E.T., p. 441, etc.; three in A.S. Reader7, p. 189, etc. Bede Glosses (MS. Cott., C. II.), circa 900, O.E.T., p. 179, etc.

4. West Saxon.Works of King Alfred: Cura Pastoralis, Sweet, 1871; Orosius, Sweet, 1880. Parker MS. of Anglo-Saxon Chronicle down to 891, Ed. Plummer. Two of the Saxon Chronicles. 2 vols. Oxford, 1892-1900.

LATE O.E. DOCUMENTS

C. Late Texts.

1. Northumbrian
Northern areaDurham Ritual: Surtees Soc., vol. iv., 1840. Cf. also Skeat’s collation, Tr. Phil. Soc., 1879. Durham Book or Lindisfarne Gospels: Skeat, Gospels in Anglo-Saxon, 1871-1887.

1. Northumbrian
Southern areaRushworth MS: Interlinear version of SS. Mark, Luke, John, known as Rushworth², Matthew in this MS. being in Mercian. Cf. Skeat’s ed. of Gospels above.

2. Mercian.Rushworth²: Interlinear Gloss to Matthew, second half of tenth century. Cf. Skeat above. Glosses from MS. Royal, 2 A. 20. Ed. by Zupitza in Zeitschrift für deutsches Altertum, Bd. xxxiii., p. 47, etc. (circa 1000).

3. Kentish.Glosses: Zupitza in Ztschr. f. d. A., xxi., p. 1, etc., and xxii., p. 223, etc.; also in Wright-Wülker’s Vocabularies, p. 55, etc., 1884. Hymn, known as ‘Kentish Hymn,’ in Kluge’s ags Lesebuch and Sweet’s A.S. Reader. Psalm L., known as ‘Kentish Psalm,’ in Kluge’s Lesebuch.

4. West Saxon.Ælfric’s Grammar and Glossary (circa 1000), Zupitza, 1880. Ælfric’s Homilies, Ed. Thorpe, 1844-1846. West Saxon Gospels, MS. Corpus, Cambridge (written at Bath, circa 1000). Cf. Skeat’s Ed. of Gospels in Anglo-Saxon above.

5. Another Saxon Dialect, but not the West Saxon of Ælfred nor of Ælfric, is represented by a Gloss. (Harleian MS. 3,376; printed Wright-Wülker, 1, 192, etc.) and a set of Homilies, known as the Blickling Homilies (Ed. Morris, E.E.T.S., 1880). Both of these texts are tenth century, the latter MS. being dated 979 in the text itself.

Authorities on O.E. Grammar.—The best general authorities on O.E. Grammar are Bülbring, Altenglisches Elementarbuch, Heidelberg, 1902; and Sievers, Angelsächsische Grammatik, Halle, 1898. These works deal with all the problems of O.E. Grammar, the latter entering into the discussion of dialectal differences with considerable minuteness. A brief but reliable outline is found in the Grammatical Introduction to Sweet’s Anglo-Saxon Reader, seventh edition.

The following special monographs will be found useful for advanced, detailed study of O.E. dialects:

Northumbrian Texts.

Lindelöf, V.: Die Sprache d. Rituals von Durham, Helsingfors, 1890. Wörterbuch zur interlinearglosse des Rituale Ecclesiae Dunelmensis, Bonner Beiträge zur Anglistic ix., 1901. Die Südnorthumbrischen Mundart (Die Spr. d. gl. Rushworth²), Bonner Beitr., x., 1901. Glossar zur altnorthumbrischen Evangelienberzetzung die sogenannte Glosse Rushworth,² Helsingfors, 1897.

Lea, E. M.: The Language of the Northumbrian Gloss to the Gospel of St. Mark, Anglia, xvi., 62-206.

Füchsel, H.: Die Sprache d. northumbrischen interlinearversion zum Johannes-Evangelium, Anglia, xxiv., MONOGRAPHS ON O.E. DIALECTS 1-99.

[Both of the above, Lea and Füchsel, deal with the Lindisfarne Gospels, or Durham Book.]

Cook, A. S.: A Glossary of the Old Northumbrian Gospels (Lindisfarne), Halle, 1894.

Mercian Texts.

Dieter, F.: Die Sprache und Mundart, der ältesten englischen Denkmäler (Espinal and Corpus Glossaries), Göttingen, 1885.

Chadwick, H. M.: Studies in Old English (deals with the old Glossaries), 1899.

Brown, E. M.: Spr. d. Rushworth Glossen (Rushw.¹), Part I., Göttingen, 1891. The Language of the Rushworth Gloss to Matthew, Part II., Göttingen, 1892.

Zeuner, R.: Die Spr. d. Kentischen Psalters (Vespas. A. 1), Halle, 1881.

[This text (Vespasian Psalter) was formerly supposed to be Kentish, though now universally recognised as Mercian.]

Thomas, P. G., and Wyld, H. C.: A Glossary of the Mercian Hymns (in Vespas. A. 1) in Otia Merseiana, vol. iv., Liverpool, 1904.

Grimm, C.: Glossar. z. Vesp. Ps. und d. Hymnen, Heidelberg, 1906.

Kentish Texts.

Wolf, R.: Untersuchung d. Laute in d. Kentischen Urkunden, Heidelberg, 1893.

Williams, Irene: Grammatical Investigation of the Old Kt. Glosses (MS. Vespas. D. vi.), Bonner Beitr., xix., 1906.

West Saxon.

THE OLD ENGLISH

PERIOD

Cosijn, P. J.: Altwestsächsische Grammatik, Haag, 1888.

[This is practically an exhaustive monograph based upon Alfred’s Cura Pastoralis. It treats also, though less fully, with the forms of the Parker Chronicle. It is invaluable for the study of Early West Saxon.]

Fischer, F.: The Stressed Vowels of Alfric’s Homilies. Publications of Mod. Lang. Assoc. of America, vol. i., Baltimore, 1889.

Brüll, H.: Die altenglische Latein-Grammatik des Alfric, Berlin, 1904.

Trilsbach, G.: Die Lautlehre d. spätwestsächsischen Evangelien, Bonn, 1905.

Harris, M. A.: Glossary of the West Saxon Gospels, Boston, 1899.

Saxon Patois.

Hardy: Die Sprache d. Blickling-Homilien, Leipzig, 1899.

Boll, P.: Die Sprache d. altenglischen Glossen in Ms Harley 3,376, Bonner Beitr. xv., 1904.

Numerous articles on special points are referred to in the works here enumerated, and in the grammars of Sievers and Bülbring.

Pronunciation of Old English.

This is established by the following considerations: (1) Old English was first written, after the introduction of Christianity, in the British form of the Latin alphabet. The contemporary pronunciation of Latin is therefore important in settling the probable value of the symbols in O.E., since the English would naturally use the symbol which represented in Latin the nearest sound to PRONUNCIATION OF VOWELS their own. (2) Phonetic considerations based (a) upon the West Germanic origin of the English sound, (b) upon the subsequent history of the sound in Middle and Modern English. (3) A comparison of varieties of spelling of the same word, representing different scribal attempts to express the same sound, or unconscious lapses from the traditional mode of spelling, in favour of one more phonetic. (4) Accents in the manuscripts indicating quantity; length is also sometimes expressed by doubling the vowel.

In spite of everything, however, there must always remain some uncertainty and difference of opinion on certain points.

The following table shows the probable value of the O.E. symbols of the vowels:

Unrounded Vowels.Rounded Vowels.
Back.Front.Back.Front.
Highī̆ū̆ȳ̆
Midaē̆ō̆œ̄̆ (< ē̆)
Lowā (or mid?)ǣ̆

There are also combinations of above in the diphthongs e̅͝a̅, e̅͝u̅ (e̅͝o̅ <); i̅͝u̅ (< W.S. i̅͝e̅ or e̅͝o̅; Kt. e̅͝o̅ or i̅͝o̅; North, i̅o̅; Mer. e̅͝o̅). [The marks of length are only occasional in the manuscripts.]

As regards the question of whether the above vowels were ‘tense’ or ‘slack,’ it is probable that the High and Mid vowels in the front series (unrounded) existed in a ‘tense’ form, both long and short, and, further, that a short mid-front-slack also existed, having a different origin. It is usual among English scholars to write this vowel , a symbol which is found in some manuscripts.

The symbol o̅͝e̅ (mid-front-round) hardly occurs in W. Saxon texts, ē̆ being the symbol used already in Early W. Saxon. This probably implies that unrounding took place earlier in this dialect than in the others. In Northumbrian œ is used during the whole O.E. period. On the whole, it is possible that all the round vowels were tense.

Originally, doubtless, (ɔ̄) low-back-tense-round, and the same vowel short and slack, existed, but the long at any rate seems to have been levelled under the mid-back-round, by, or soon after, the historic period.

Pronunciation of Old English Consonants.

In addition to the ordinary Latin consonantal symbols, certain letters of Runic origin are habitually used from the ninth century onwards to express English sounds which did not exist in Latin. Thus þ (‘thorn’) is written to express the point-teeth-open consonant, whether voiced or voiceless, and ƿ (‘wēn’) to express that of ‘w’ (lip-back-open).

Before the historic period, the old k (back-stop-breath) was differentiated in O.E. into a back and a front stop. The latter was the ancestor of the Mod. Eng. ‘ch’-sound (tʃ). The manuscripts occasionally write k for the former, but more often c, which does duty both for the back and the front sounds. It is convenient to distinguish the two sounds by writing ċ for the fronted consonant. It is a disputed point how soon the full (tʃ) sound, as in Present PRONUNCIATION OF ċ, g, ġ, ċġ IN O.E. English, developed. Most German scholars insist that this sound was fully established quite early in the O.E. period. Sweet has always held that the O.E. sound was a front stop, which view is shared by the present writer. It is merely a question of probabilities, and cannot be definitely settled one way or the other. The really important thing is to realize that there were two sounds in O.E., a back and a front, and to express this fact in pronunciation.

Another symbol whose pronunciation is doubtful is g. The O.E. form of this letter is always ʓ, or ȝ, down to the middle of the eleventh century, after which the Continental g is used. There were originally two sounds in West Gmc., which were inherited by O.E.,and expressed by the symbol ȝ, etc., a back-open-voice and front-open-voice, (i.e., j). The back-open, before the historical period, was differentiated into a back and a front sound, the latter thus being levelled under original j to all appearances. These sounds continue to be written ȝ without any distinction during the O.E. period. It is probable that by the year 1000, or thereabouts, the back-open was stopped initially, but remained an open consonant medially and finally.

The O.E. symbol, cȝ, which represents the doubling of old g before j, was, in Sweet’s view, pronounced as a voiced front stop during the O.E. period. Here again opinions are divided, German scholars, Sievers, Bülbring, and Kluge, maintaining that the Mod. Eng. sound -‘dge’ (dž) was already established.

For a full account and discussion of O.E. pronunciation, cf. Bülbring, Elementarbuch, pp. 13-31; Sweet, History of English Sounds, pp. 101-149; and for an additional discussion of O.E. c, g, cg, also Kluge in Paul’s Grundriss, pp. 989, etc.

The most practical book for beginners who want to learn the language is probably Sweet’s First Steps in Anglo-Saxon, which should be followed up with his Anglo-Saxon Reader (seventh edition). Both works contain a short, practical account of the pronunciation, a practical grammar, accidence and syntax, as well as well-chosen texts, and a glossary. Another book, which may be recommended to beginners is A. S. Cook’s First Book in Old English, Athenæum Press, 1903 (third edition), which, in addition to phonology, grammar, vocabulary, and texts, contains also a useful bibliography.

Old English Sound Changes.

The vowel system of O.E. is distinguished from that of the other West Gmc. languages, notably from Old High German, by a number of characteristic changes which took place in the former group of dialects, mostly before the period of the documents. These changes are of both the Isolative and Combinative classes, and a knowledge of them is of importance to those who wish to pursue the history of the language in a systematic way, further back than Old English itself, and to inquire into its precise relationship with the other West Gmc. languages.

For those whose main object, however, is to trace the growth of the Modern Language, and to relate it to the earlier forms, a detailed knowledge of the minutiæ of O.E. sound change is out of place for this particular purpose.

In the same way, the specialist is deeply interested in the dialectal differences of O.E. The most important of ISOLATIVE SOUND CHANGES these consist in the different treatment, in different geographical areas, of the original vowel sounds. But these early differences are but faintly reflected, even in the full M.E. period of the language, and in the Modern speech hardly any of the primitive dialectal distinctions can be traced.

The various local treatment of sounds which we find in M.E. seems in the light of our present knowledge of O.E. to be but of recent growth, and as for the English dialects of to-day, their peculiarities, so far as we can trace their origin, would appear for the most part not to be more than two, or at the most three, hundred years old.

As in a work like the present space is necessarily limited, it will be best in dealing with the phonology of O.E. to consider mainly, such typical sound changes, whether of common O.E. origin or subsequently developed during the O.E. period, within the several dialects, as have left their traces upon the language of the present day, of which some knowledge is necessary in order to understand the phenomena of Mod. Eng. grammar. For this purpose we shall endeavour to make a judicious selection in the following account.

Changes in the West Germanic Vowels which affected Old English generally.

A. Isolative Changes.

1. W. Gmc. a < O.E. œ: O.E. dœġ; Gothic dag-s; O.H.G. tac; O.E. æcer, ‘field’; O. Sax. akkar; O.H.G. acchar.

2. W. Gmc. ā < O.E. ǣ: O.E. mǣþ, ‘mowing’; O.H.G. mād; O.E. wǣpn, ‘weapon’; O.H.G. wāfan.

3. W. Gmc. ã (i.e., nasalized a) < õ, then, with loss of nasalization, O.E. ō: þōhte, pret. of þenċan, from þāhta, cf. Goth. þāhta; O.H.G. dāhta, ‘thought.’

[Note.—This nasalized ã, which was developed already in Germanic itself (cf. under Combinative Changes, pp. 231-233), appears rounded to ō in the earliest English texts, of all dialects. It is probable that originally it was a low-back-tense-round, though it may have been raised to the mid position quite early.]

4. W. Gmc. ai < O.E. ā: O.E. hām; Goth. haims; O.H.G. heim; O.E. gāt, ‘goat’; Goth. gaits; O.H.G. geiz.

5. W. Gmc. au < O.E. ǣū, whence ǣō, ǣā, and finally ēā in nearly all dialects: O.E. ēāge, ‘eye’; Goth. augō; O.H.G. ouga; O.E. hēāfod, ‘head’; Goth. haubiþ; O.H.G. houbit.

B. Combinative Changes.

1. Rounding of W. Gmc. a to o before Nasals.—In O.E. texts of all periods, from ninth century onwards, such double forms as mann, monn, land, lond, nama, noma, ‘name,’ etc., are found. The oldest texts have only -an- in these words, and a comparison with the other Gmc. languages leaves no doubt that this is the original form. In ninth-century texts, however (King Alfred’s period), the forms with -on- largely predominate, while later on, in the tenth and eleventh centuries, those with -an- are again in the majority.

In M.E. the -on- forms again become frequent, but in Mod. Eng. they have almost entirely disappeared, the preposition on being the only form which has survived in the polite language, apart from cases where lengthening ROUNDING OF Ā̆ IN O.E. has taken place (see below).

It might appear that such words as ‘strong,’ ‘long,’ etc., were examples of the preservation of the -on- forms; but this, as we shall see, is not the case, and these forms require a different explanation (see p. 273).

It is impossible to believe in the alternate change of -an- to -on-, and of this to -an- in late O.E., and again of this back to -on- in M.E., and finally in a return to -an- in Mod. Eng. At any rate, there cannot have been an alternate process of rounding and unrounding going on for centuries. As Sweet pointed out long ago (see Introduction to Cura Pastoralis, p. xxii), in all dialects, at all periods, both -an- and -on- forms are found; sometimes one is in the majority, sometimes the other. It looks as if a double pronunciation existed at the same time amid speakers of the same dialect, just as nowadays we hear both (æs) and (ās) = ‘ass,’ and so on, among persons who otherwise have no dialectal peculiarity. The preponderance of this or that form may have been quite artificial, and a question of fashion.

2. Rounding of W. Gmc. ā to ō before Nasals.—This is universal in all O.E. dialects from the earliest period. Examples are: O.E. mōna, ‘moon’; O. Sax. and O.H.G. māno; O.E. nōmon, pret. pl. of niman, ‘take’; O.H.G. nāmum, etc. This sound (ā), as we have seen, otherwise than before nasals, becomes œ̄ in O.E., and its subsequent non-W. Sax. development is important in the history of the language.

3. Fracture or ‘Brechung.’—This is the name given to the diphthonging of original O.E. front vowels before certain consonants or combinations of consonants. This change is not, in all its forms, strictly ‘common O.E.,’ since it is more fully developed in W. Sax. and Kentish than in the Anglian dialects. The dialectal differences in this particular will, however, be discussed subsequently, and we may now content ourselves with describing the process itself, and the conditions under which it occurs in those dialects in which it is most observable.

The Primitive O.E. front vowels i, e, œ are diphthongized respectively to iu, eu, and œu before h or h + another consonant, rr or r + another consonant; œ undergoes the same change before ll or l + another consonant, and i, e before l + h or c.

The process depends upon the character of the following consonants: h was a back-open-voiceless, and ll, rr, or l and r, when followed by other consonants, appear to have been pronounced either as back consonants, or, as is more probable, as strongly inverted consonants—that is, with the point of the tongue turned upwards and backwards. This mode of articulation is heard to-day in the pronunciation of r throughout the whole of the Saxon part of England, and also in Oxfordshire. Inverted l, or l formed with considerable hollowing out of the front part of the tongue, is also common in the Southern dialects. The result of this method of articulation was that a strong glide vowel was developed between i, e, œ, and the following h, ll, etc., and rr, etc. At the present day in such a word as ‘ale’ we often hear (aiᵘl) with a fairly distinct u-like glide before the ‘thick’ l.

The glide in O.E. would appear to have been of u quality. In the ninth century œu had become ea, and eu eo—in West Saxon at any rate. In an early Northumbrian FRACTURE—LOSS OF NASALS text (Bede’s Death Song) iu is still preserved in wiurþiþ, later wiorþeþ.

Examples are:

(1) of œ: O.E. (W.S. and Kt.) eahta, ‘eight,’ O. Sax., O.H.G. ahto; O.E. earm, ‘poor,’ O.H.G. arm; O.E. (W.S. and Kt.) ceald, ‘cold,’ O.H.G. kalt.

(2) of e: O.E. feohtan, ‘fight,’ vb., O.H.G. fehtan; O.E. eorþe ‘earth,’ O. Sax. ertha, O.H.G. erda; O.E. colh, ‘elk,’ cf. M.H.G. elch.

4. Loss of Nasal Consonant before Voiceless Open Consonants (h, f, þ, s), and the Result on Preceding Vowel.—(a) Before h: Since all the Gmc. languages show a loss of n and m before a following h, we may assume that this loss took place in the common Gmc. period. Before disappearing, however, the nasal consonant nasalized the preceding vowel, and in O.E., at any rate, the nasalization was preserved down to the beginning of the English period. Examples: Goth. þagkjan (= þaŋkjan), ‘think,’ pret. þāhta; O.H.G. denken, dâchta, with originally nasalized ã. The preterite form is from earlier *þaŋk-ta, which became *þaŋh-ta, with the common Gmc. change of -kt- to -ht-. The O.E. form þōhte shows the characteristic rounding of this nasal vowel, and compensatory lengthening after the loss of nasalization. The Primitive O.E. distinction between this ã and W. Gmc. ā is shown by the difference of the subsequent treatment in O.E., the latter being fronted to œ̄.

Another example of this rounding and lengthening in O.E. is brōhte, pret. of bring-an, which stands for earlier *braŋhta, which became *brãhta. Other vowels than a are merely lengthened in compensation for the loss of nasality; thus O.E. þūhte, pret. of þynċean, ‘seem,’ from þūhte, from *þuŋhta; O.E. þēōn, ‘prosper,’ is from *þiŋhan, which in Prim. O.E. was *þīhan, whence *þīūhan, with Fracture, which in W. Sax. became *þiu(h)an, *þīon, and finally þēōn, with change of īō < ēō. In O. Sax. this vb. appears as thīhan, and in O.H.G. dīhan. The original n is seen in another form preserved in O.E., geþungen (originally a participial form), in which earlier h has been voiced to g (back-open-voice) by the process known as Verner’s Law, which depends upon the place of the accent. Before g the nasal consonant is not lost.

(b) Loss of Nasal before f, þ, s.—This is a Primitive Old English change, but is precisely similar in nature and in results to the foregoing.

O.E. sōfte, ‘soft,’ O.H.G. samfto; O.E. tōþ, ‘tooth’; O.H.G. zand, both from earlier *tanþ (see ante, pp. 152-3); O.E. sīþ, ‘journey,’ Goth. sinþs, O.H.G. sind; O.E. gōs, ‘goose,’ O.H.G. gans; O.E. ūs, ‘us,’ O.H.G. uns.

It is probable that the ō in these words, as well as in the class before mentioned, which show an earlier loss of the nasal, was originally different from the other O.E. ō (in fōt, ‘foot,’ etc.), which represents an original Gmc. ō. The former may have been the low-back-round. In any case, there is no graphic distinction made between the two sounds in O.E., and their subsequent history has been identical. The levelling under one sound almost certainly took place early in the O.E. period.

In words like O.E. gōs, tōp, etc., the process of change was apparently as follows: *gans, *gãns, *gãs, *gõs, gōs. The rounding of the nasalized ã was earlier than THE O.E. PROCESS OF I-MUTATION that of a before a nasal consonant, since the earliest texts invariably have ō in gōs, etc., whereas, as we have seen, monn, etc., appear in the earliest records of English with a.

5. i- or j- Mutation.—This process, often called by the German name, i-Umlaut, is common to all the O.E. dialects, and there is no O.E. sound change whose traces are so perceptible in Mod. Eng. It consists in the fronting of an original back vowel, or diphthong, which contained at least one back element, by the influence of a following -i- or -j- in the following syllable. It is generally held now that the -i- or -j- first fronted or front-modified the intervening consonant or group of consonants, and that this in turn fronted the vowel immediately preceding them.[H] The only front vowel affected is œ, which is raised to e. In this case it was possible for the fronting of the vowel not to take place until after the i or j had disappeared altogether. All that was necessary was that, before being dropped, it should have fronted to a greater or lesser extent the intervening consonant. The fronting of the vowel was a comparatively late process, taking place about the beginning of the seventh century, shortly before the earliest manuscripts which we possess in O.E. were written. It can be shown that i-mutation was later than Fracture, for instance, since diphthongs produced by the latter process are further affected by the former. In cases where the -i- or -j- have disappeared in O.E. its original existence can usually be established by referring to the cognate word in Gothic or Old High German.


When the fronting was caused by -j- as in -ja- or -jo-stem nouns or -jan verbs, the -j- was assimilated to the preceding consonant, which was thus not only fronted, but lengthened—as in cynn, from *kunja, etc. r was not doubled, and -j- remained (after short vowels). When final, -j- became -i- and the e in O.E. Cf. here > heri > *hærj > *harja.

The following examples illustrate the effect of this mutation upon the various vowels:

The mutation ofœ is e: O.E. þeċċean, ‘to cover,’ from *þœkk-jan (cf. O.E. þœc, ‘roof’).
The mutation ofa is æ: O.E. ġe-slæġen, ‘struck,’ p.p. from *slag-in-.
The mutation ofo is e (earlier œ): O.E. ele, ‘oil,’ loan-word from Latin oleum, W. Gmc. *olja.
The mutation ofw is y: O.E. cynn, ‘race,’ ‘family,’ from *kuṅṅj, cf. Gothic kuni from *kunja.
O.E. fyllan, ‘fill,’ from *fulljan (cf. O. E. full).
The mutation ofā is ǣ: O.E. sǣlan, ‘bind,’ from *sāljan (cf. O.E. sāljan, ‘rope’).
The mutation ofō is ē (earlier œ̄): 1. Original ō: O.E. fēt, from *fōtiz, pl. of O.E. fōt.
2. ō from õ: O.E. gēs, pl. of gōs, from *gõsi.
3. ō from W. Gmc. ã: O.E. fēhþ, ‘takes,’ from *fōhiþ, *fõhiþ, *faŋhiþ (cf. O.E. , ‘I take,’ from *fōha, *fāha, *faŋha).
The mutation ofū is ȳ: 1. W. Gmc. ū: O.E. fȳlþ, ‘filth,’ from *fūliþ, O. Sax. fūliþa (cf. O.E. fūl, ‘foul’).
2. O.E. ū: O.E. dȳstiġ, ‘dusty,’ from *dũstig (cf. O.E. dūst, O.H.G. dunst).

The i-mutation of the O.E. diphthongs will be best treated under the head of Dialectal Divergences.

In some words it might appear that y was the mutation of oe.g., gylden, ‘golden,’ compared with gold, the substantive; fyxen, ‘vixen,’ feminine of fox; gyden, ‘goddess,’ compared with god. The fact is that the o in the above words is a W. Gmc. change from an earlier u before a following a in the stem ending. The original u was, however, preserved unchanged when followed by i, so that *gulđin-, *fuhsin-, *guđin, remained unchanged until the period when the following -i- fronted the root vowel to y.

Lengthening of Short Vowels.—During the O.E. period VOWEL LENGTHENING IN O.E. original short vowels were lengthened before the consonantal combinations -ld, nd, mb: ċīld, ‘child’; fīndan, vb. ‘find’; cāmb, ‘comb.’ These lengthenings are important for the subsequent history of the language, their later development being similar to that of original long vowels. When these combinations are followed by another consonant, such as r, which occurs, for instance, in the plural suffix, -rucĭldru, lămbru, etc.—the lengthening does not take place, or is subsequently got rid of. This explains the interchange of diphthong and short vowel in (tʃaild—tʃildrən), and also the short vowel in Mod. Eng. (læm), which must be explained from the plural type with a short vowel in O.E.

Many later shortenings took place in cases where a third consonant follows the vowel in compounds e.g., hānd, hăndfull, etc. (cf. p. 272, etc., below).

Dialectal Divergences in the Old English Vowel System.

Each of the O.E. dialects possesses certain characteristic phonological features peculiar to itself alone. The West Saxon dialect has more individual peculiarities than any of the others which, in a large number of cases, agree in those respects in which they differ from West Saxon. Thus it is often sufficient to describe a characteristic as West Saxon on the one hand, or as non-West Saxon on the other, implying by the latter phrase that Northumbrian, Mercian, and Kentish agree in that particular respect.

In Modern English it is comparatively rare that a form can be derived only from the exclusively West Saxon type, though this sometimes happens. On the other hand, the survivals of Anglian peculiarities, common to both Northumbria and Mercia, are numerous; a few specifically Northumbrian, exist, and a few which are specifically Kentish.

The following are the chief O.E. dialectal differences which can still be traced in Modern Polite English:

A. Features Common to all the non-West Saxon Dialects.—1. Primitive O.E. œ̄, which remains in W.S., is raised to ē in the other dialects: W.S. dœ̄d, ‘deed,’ non-W.S. dēd; W.S., sœ̄d, ‘seed,’ non-W.S. sēd. The forms with ē are the ancestral forms of the Mod. Eng. (ī) forms, seed, deed, etc. The other O.E. œ̄, the i-mutation of ā, is preserved in all dialects except Kentish, which raises it to ē: clēne, ‘clean’; in other dialects clœ̄ne, from *clāni.

2. The i-mutation of Pr. O.E. e̅a̅ (Gmc. au) is i̅e̅, later ȳ in W.S.; but in the other dialects ē: W.S. hi̅e̅ran, later hȳran, ‘hear,’ from *he̅a̅rjan. Cf. Goth. hausjan > Gmc. *hauzjan, non-W.S. hēran. This is the origin of Mod. Eng. ‘hear’ (hiə(r)). The W.S. form, had it survived, would have given (haiə(r)).

3. After front consonants, (ċ, ġ, ), ǣ̆, and e are diphthongized, in W.S., to e̅a̅, and ie (later y) respectively. This diphthonging does not take place in non-W.S.—e.g., sċeld, ‘shield,’ W.S. sċīeld, sċȳld; non-W.S. scēld, whence Mod. Eng. (ʃīld). On the other hand, Mod. Eng. chill is apparently from W.S. ċi(e)le, and not from non-W.S. ċele. The W.S. form is from *ċœli, whence *ċeali, and then ċiele, ċyle, with i-mutation of ea.

B. Common Anglian Features.—1. Pr. O.E. a, æ is not diphthongized to ea before l, ll, or l + another consonant, in Anglian as in W.S., but remains as a, and is subsequently lengthened to ā: W.S. eald, ‘old,’ Ang. āld; W.S. ċeald, ‘cold,’ Anglian cāld; W.S. beald, ‘bold,’ Anglian bāld; W.S. weald, ‘forest,’ Anglian wāld. The long ā in these CHARACTERISTICS OF THE VARIOUS DIALECTS words, together with all other O.E. ā sounds, was rounded to ō in M.E. in the South and Midlands, and is the origin of Mod. Eng. (ou). Thus the Anglian forms of above words gave rise to Mod. Eng. old, cold, bold, wold. The W.S. form of the last word appears to be also preserved in the modern doublet form weald.

C. Distinctively Northumbrian Features.—1. In Late Northumbrian the combination weo- appears as wo-. The same combination in Late W.S. appears as wu: W.S. weorþ, later wurþ, Late Nth. worþ; W.S. sweord, ‘sword,’ later swurd, Late Nth. sword, etc. Mercian and Kentish preserve weo unaltered. 2. i̅͝u̅ does not undergo change to e̅͝o̅, but preserves the first element unaltered during O.E. period.

D. Kentish Features.—In Kentish, by the middle of the ninth century, the earlier ȳ̆}-sounds, the result of i-mutation of ū̆, had been unrounded and lowered to ē̆. All the other dialects preserve ȳ̆ during the whole O.E. period. In M.E., as we shall see, the Saxon dialects alone preserved the old sound; the Anglian unrounded it to ī̆. Thus, such forms as gelt, ‘guilt,’ W.S. gylt; senn, ‘sin,’ W.S. synn; snetor, ‘wise,’ W.S. snytor, etc., are typically Kentish. In the modern language a few of these forms with old Kentish e occur—e.g., merry, from Kentish meriġ = W.S. myriġ. The cognate substantive mirth, on the other hand, is Anglian as regards its spelling, while the actual pronunciation might be from either the W.S. or the Anglian type. In a few cases the modern forms preserve the M.E. spelling u, which is Norman French manner of expressing the old Saxon y sound—e.g., church, from W.S. ċyrċe; bury (vb.), W.S. byrġean, M.E. (Southern) burien. In the latter word it is interesting to note that, although we retain the Southern (Saxon) spelling, we pronounce the Kentish vowel e (bϵri). Such words as ridge and bridge, O.E. hryċġ, bryċġ, are Middle Anglian in spelling and pronunciation, but the Southern or Saxon variants occur in dialectal forms, such as Somersetshire burge, with metathesis, and in proper names, such as Rudge.

[Note.—The original O.E. form of ċyrċe is ċir(i)ċe; the y, which is represented by M.E. u, must be due to the influence of r.]

The Old English Vocabulary.

The native vocabulary closely agrees with that of the other W. Gmc. languages, and more particularly with that of the Continental Angles, with O. Frisian and O. Saxon. The foreign elements are, in the main, from three sources, Celtic, Latin, and Old Norse.

Celtic Loan-Words in Old English.

The number of these is far smaller than was formerly supposed, and it is probable that a thorough investigation of Welsh would reveal the existence of a larger number of words borrowed from English in the early period into that language.

Among those words of undoubted Celtic origin which are found in O.E., it is possible to distinguish at least two strata: those which were passed into the vocabulary during the common Germanic period, and which survived in the several Germanic languages after the separation, and those which came independently into the English vocabulary through contact of the Germanic settlers in these islands with the Celtic inhabitants.

One of the earliest of the former class is O.E. rīce, CELTIC LOAN-WORDS ‘kingdom,’ ‘rule,’ which is found also in Gothic reiki, ‘kingdom,’ reiks, ‘ruler,’ O.S. rīki, O.H.G. rīhhi (Mod. Germ. reich). This word in the form *rīg- must have been borrowed from Celtic sources before the Pr. Gmc. ‘shifting’ of the original voiced stops b, d, g, to p, t, k; hence the g was unvoiced along with the original Aryan voiced stops. In O. Irish the word is , with genitive rīg, which is cognate with Latin rēx (rēk-s, from *rēg-s) and reg-o, etc. Mod. Eng. still preserves the word in bishop-ric.

Other words for which this Pr. Celtic origin is sometimes claimed are doubtful, since, instead of being loan-words borrowed before the Germanic consonant ‘shifting,’ they may equally well be cognates possessed by Germanic and Celtic alike.

Among words borrowed in Britain in the O.E. period may be mentioned drȳ, ‘magician,’ in common use in poetry, borrowed, apparently, from a form resembling that found in O. Irish drui. Mod. Eng. druid is related to this word, but has reached us through the French, from Gaulish sources. Another word is O.E. dunn, ‘dun,’ ‘dark brown,’ from a Celtic type, donnas. Cf. Welsh dwn (= dun), ‘dusky,’ Irish donn, ‘brown.’ Brocc, ‘badger’ (cf. O. Ir. brocc), occurs already in the Epinal Glossary, and is still in dialectal use.

Latin Element in Old English.

This forms by far the most considerable part of the foreign element in the O.E. vocabulary. The question is not so simple as might appear from the lists of Latin loan-words which are given in some books on the history of English. It is possible to distinguish at least three classes of words of Latin origin in O.E: (1) Words which formed part of the common West Germanic, or common Germanic, vocabulary; (2) words acquired first in this country, before the conversion of the English to Christianity; (3) words which passed into O.E. at a later period, after the introduction of Christianity, through the influence of the Church and the spread of learning.

The only true test of the period at which any particular word was borrowed is its form. It is certain that some words relating to Christian ideas and beliefs were adopted by the Germanic peoples long before they were converted from heathendom; while, as is natural, the actual adoption of the Christian religion, its forms and ceremonies, its ideals and its culture, led to the introduction of a host of fresh words to express new ideas. It is therefore unsound and inaccurate to mix up in one class all the words of Latin origin which relate to Christianity, and label them ‘words of Christian origin.’ O.E. cẏrċe, ċiriċe, ‘church,’ from Gk. κυριακά, ‘belonging to the Lord,’ is a very early loan, which goes back at least to the W. Gmc. period (cf. O.H.G. chirihha.)

1. As regards the earliest class of Latin words, those acquired in the Continental Period, it is possible that some may have passed into W. Gmc. through the medium of Celtic; and, again, it is not always possible, apparently, even for Celtic experts, to distinguish with absolute certainty between words in Celtic which are Latin loan-words and those which are genuine Celtic, cognate with the Latin forms.

The best tests of a Latin word having been adopted in the Gmc. or W. Gmc. period are, first, the retention in genuine LATIN WORDS FROM CONTINENTAL PERIOD popular words of the Latin intervocalic i, t, c (k), unaffected by the later Neo-Latin voicing: O.E. nœ̄p, ‘turnip,’ Lat. nāpus; mynet, ‘coin,’ Lat. moneta; fi̅c̅-be̅a̅m, ‘fig-tree,’ Lat. fi̅c̅us; secondly, its occurrence in several Gmc. tongues with the characteristic treatment which it would have undergone in each language had it belonged to the native element of Gmc. or W. Gmc. Thus O.E. strǣt, compared with O. Sax. strāta, O.H.G. strāzza, Mod. Eng. street, from Latin strāta via, ‘paved way,’ clearly belonged to the common W. Gmc. vocabulary, for the ā has been fronted to œ̄ in O.E. like original W. Gmc. ā, and the O.H.G. form shows the High German change of W. Gmc. t to zz. In the same way O.E. (W. Sax.) ċi̅e̅se, later ċȳse, non-W. Sax. ċēse, is a W. Gmc. loan from Latin cāseus, whence we may assume a form *kāsjō-, *kāsi, which gave rise on the one hand to O.H.G. chāsi (Mod. Germ, käse), and on the other to the English forms. (W. Sax. ċi̅e̅se is from earlier *ċeāsi, from *cǣsi, with diphthongization of ǣ to e̅a̅ after a front consonant, and subsequent i-mutation to i̅e̅, whence ȳ in Late W. Sax.) Mod. Eng. ‘cheese’ is from the non-W. Sax. form. Latin Cæsar was adopted into Gmc. speech at an early period, the sound of the old diphthong being approximately preserved: Gothic kaisar, O.H.G. cheisar. In O.E. the diphthong underwent, in common with W. Gmc. ai, the characteristic change to ā; hence we get O.E. cāsere. It is, of course, possible that this word was independently borrowed by Gothic and by W. Gmc.

It must be borne in mind that in these loan-words we are not dealing with words written down, with the spelling of classical Latin, but with words actually used in living popular speech. In popular Latin, b between vowels was early weakened to an open consonant, at first a pure lip-open, like Gmc. Ѣ. This sound is generally written f in O.E., though the spelling b is found in early texts. In O.H.G. it is written b; hence Lat. cucurbita, ‘gourd,’ O.E. cyrfet (with i-mutation), O.H.G. churbizz; Lat. tabula, ‘plank,’ ‘writing-table,’ O.E. tæfl, ‘table’ (for games), O.H.G. zabal, and so on.

2. Words from Popular Sources acquired in Britain.—Wright, in his The Celt, the Roman, and the Saxon, propounded the view that the people in the towns in this country continued to speak Latin long after the Romans had withdrawn from the island, and expresses his belief that if Britain had not been settled by the English ‘we should have been now a people talking a Neo-Latin tongue, closely resembling French.’ He thinks that the Angles and Saxons found the inhabitants of this country speaking Latin, and not a Celtic dialect. Pogatscher, in his important book, Zur Lautlehre der Griechischen und Lateinischen und Romanischen Lehnworte im Altenglischen, 1888, accepts this view in the fullest possible way, going further, indeed, than Wright, who, in the passage quoted by Pogatscher himself (loc. cit., p. 3), expressly says: ‘I have a strong suspicion, from different circumstances I have remarked, that the towns in our island continued, in contradistinction from the country, to use the Latin tongue long after the Empire of Rome had disappeared, and after the country had become Saxon.’ Subsequently, however, Pogatscher’s views were, to a certain extent, modified by the arguments of Loth (Les Mots Latins dans les Langues Brittoniques, 1892), and in an article, Angellsachsen und Romanen LATIN WORDS ACQUIRED FROM BRITISH SPEAKERS (Englische Studien, xix., p. 3, etc.), he apparently contents himself with Wright’s view that Latin was spoken in cities, without insisting that it had become the national language. The important point, however, is that it seems to be well established that a form of Latin—a popular dialect which had begun to undergo some of the changes characteristic of the Neo-Latin languages—actually was spoken in this country for some time after the coming of the Angles, Saxons, and Jutes. This form of spoken Latin was the source of the numerous popular words of Latin origin which passed into English during the period between the settlement of Britain and the acceptance of Christianity, as preached by St. Augustine. But this spoken Latin had undergone certain important changes in pronunciation by the middle of the fifth century. It no longer retained the form of old classical Latin, but had advanced in many respects in the same direction as the popular forms of Latin on the Continent, which were the ancestors of the modern Romance languages. The words borrowed from this source into O.E. had naturally already undergone the characteristic changes of early Romance, and the O.E. forms of them retain, as far as is possible, the pronunciation which they had in Brito-Romance at the date of the borrowing. When once these words had passed into O.E. speech they became part and parcel of that speech, and underwent the same subsequent changes as native O.E. words.

Among the most characteristic changes of popular Latin, which was developing into Romance, is the voicing of p, t, and c (k), between vowels. We have seen that those words borrowed from Latin in the Continental period retain the above consonants, in this position, unaltered. The later words, however, acquired in England, show a change of p to f (= v), of t to d, and of c to g. It should be noted that O.E. f represents a Romance b (voiced stop), a sound which did not occur medially in O.E. in the earliest period; g was also pronounced as an open consonant in the medial position.

Examples.—Lat. p: capistrum, ‘halter,’ O.E. cœfester, from Brit.-Rom. *kaѢestr-; prāfost, ‘officer,’ Lat. prœpositus. Lat. t: ruta, O.E. rūde, ‘rue’; moraþ, ‘sweetened wine,’ Lat. morātum, represents a further Romance development of intervocalic d from t to đ; a voiced open consonant. Lat. k: fœnuculum, O.E. finugl, ‘fennel’; Lat. cuculla, O.E. cugele, ‘cowl, monk’s hood.’

The loan-words of early Brito-Latin origin, as well, of course, as those of Continental origin, undergo, as has been said, such ordinary O.E. sound changes, as took place after the date of borrowing. A few examples are:

(1) Change of a to æ: O.E. non-W. Sax. ċæster, from *ċastr.

(2) W. Sax. diphthonging after front cons.: W. Sax. ċeaster.

(3) Fracture: Wyrtġeorn, from *Vortigern; mearm-stān, Lat. marmor; sealm, Lat. (p)salmus.

(4) i-mutation: cyċene, from Lat. coquina; Wyrtġeorn, from *Vorti- < *Wurti-.

The oldest English form of Lincoln on record is Lin(d)cylene (A. Sax. Chron., 941, 942, Parker MS.), and other manuscripts have -cylne, -kylne. Now, this, the genuine O.E. form of the Latin colonia, shows unmistakable signs of having passed through Celtic speech. CHANGES IN SPOKEN LATIN Cylene presupposes a pre-mutation form *culīne, from *colīne; the change of o to u when i follows in the next syllable being normal in O.E., and observable in many Brito-Latin loan-words. It can be shown that a change of ō to ū and of this to ȳ (high-front-round) took place in Celtic. But if this word came into English, in the place-names or otherwise, from the form *colȳ́na before the period of the O.E. i-mutation, (ȳ) would be an unknown sound to English speakers, and the nearest approach to it in English would be (ī). Hence we may assume that the earliest English form was colī́na, whence *cúlina, and finally, with mutation, cyl(e)ne. The O.E. variant -colne, whence our spelling -coln, is a later form taken direct from literary Latin.

To show how important is the form of the word in determining the date of its importation into the language, we may instance the two O.E. words ynċe, ‘inch,’ and yndse, or yntse, ‘ounce,’ which are both derived ultimately from the Latin uncia. Both show i-mutation, and must therefore both have been introduced before 600 or thereabouts. Which is the earlier form? Obviously ynċe, for the following reasons: Latin uncia, if borrowed in Gmc., would undoubtedly assume some such form as *unkjō-, which would normally become ynċe in O.E. and inch in Mod. Eng. As a matter of fact, unkja occurs in Gothic, but this may well be an independent loan. In Romance speech uncia became (*ontsja), whence later (*ontʃia), with assibilation of ċ before i, j, similar to that which developed also in English, and has given us our pronunciation (intʃ). But the English process was far slower than the Romance change; hence by the fifth or sixth centuries the latter language had already developed a sound not far removed from (tʃ), whereas O.E., although it had begun to front k before i and j, had not progressed so far. We may therefore regard the -ts- in O.E. yntse as an English approximation to the Brito-Romance sound in the word, the earlier loan ynċe having at this period probably the form (*unċi) with a front stop.

In cases where Latin words contain no test sounds such as intervocalic voiceless stops, there cannot be absolute certainty as to whether they belong to the earliest Continental class of loans, or whether they were acquired early in the English period, and even the fact that the same word exists in O.H.G. or O. Sax. does not necessarily settle the matter in favour of the former class, since each language may have adopted the words independently. On the other hand, words which retain the Latin intervocalic t, etc., might belong either to the Continental period or the late English, if their vowels are not such as are liable to early English sound changes.

Enough has perhaps been said to show that the question of Latin words in O.E. is fraught with difficulties, and one that presents some problems which cannot be definitely solved.

3. Latin Words chiefly from Ecclesiastical or Learned Sources, borrowed after Conversion of the English to Christianity.—After the introduction of the Christian religion, and with it Latin culture, into England, the vocabulary was further enriched by words both bearing directly upon the Church, its government and ideals, its officers, the functions of the ministers of religion and their vestments, etc., and also by others expressing the circumstances CHRISTIANITY IN ENGLAND and objects connected with the everyday life of Christians both clerical and lay. The new culture affected the language of Englishmen in two ways: by introducing words direct from classical Latin, and by calling into existence fresh adaptations and combination of native words to express hitherto unknown objects and ideas.

The Latin words which passed into English after the introduction of Christianity are chiefly from literary and not spoken popular Latin; hence they had not undergone the characteric changes of the latter. Again, most of the characteristic English sound changes had already been carried out by the beginning of the seventh century, so that from the English side they underwent, as a rule, comparatively little change. Further, it is probable that during the Old English period these words remained, for the most part, the linguistic property of the clergy and learned classes; they were derived from literary sources, and preserved, to a great extent, the form in which they were borrowed.

A few examples of learned words are: Discipul, ‘disciple’; martyr; pœll, ‘pallium’; pāpa, ‘pope’; sācerd, ‘priest,’ from sacerdos. Words of more popular origin and use are: Abbod, ‘abbot’; œlmesse, ‘alms,’ from alimosina; domne (applied to a Bishop or Archbishop); mœsse, ‘mass,’ from *messa, Lat. missa.

Many native words were adapted to Christian uses. Such are: hūsl, applied to the Blessed Sacrament, but originally meaning ‘sacrifice’ in general, Cf. Goth. hunsl; scearn, ‘the tonsure,’ related to scieran, ‘to cut’; ān-buend and ān-setl, ‘hermit’ and ‘hermitage’; fulwian, ‘baptize’ = *ful-wīhan, ‘consecrate’; fulluht and fulwiht, ‘baptism,’ -wiht being probably associated in popular etymology with the word meaning creature; godspellere, ‘evangelist’; hūsl-þeġn, ‘acolyte’; ġelaþung, ‘the Church’—literally, those who have received the ‘call’ or ‘invitation.’

The Picardian form market, from Latin mercātum, occurs in the Laud MS. of the Chronicle under the year 963, but this text was written in the first quarter of the twelfth century.

[In addition to the works by Kluge and Pogatscher, cited above, the reader should also consult The Influence of Christianity on the Vocabulary of Old English, Part I., by H. S. MacGillivray, Halle, 1902.]

The Scandinavian Element.

It is well known that the language of the invading Norsemen, usually known to us as the ‘Danes,’ has left considerable traces upon the vocabulary both of the literary language and of that of the dialects of English. Although the process of the blending of the two languages was undoubtedly carried out during the O.E. period, it is not until the M.E. period that this linguistic element finds its way, to any considerable extent, into the written records so far as they have come down to us. The reason for this is that for a long time English and Scandinavian were spoken side by side by two separate communities in those districts which were settled by the Northmen. Not until the two races had amalgamated, and Norse had given way altogether to English, did many Scandinavian words become part and parcel of English speech. It is pointed out by Björkman, in the introductory remarks to his EARLIEST LOANS FROM SCANDINAVIAN INVADERS excellent book, Scandinavian Loan-Words in Middle English, Part I., Halle, 1900, that the words from this source found in O.E., which, indeed, are few in number, and which have mostly died out by the M.E. period, refer for the most part to things connected with the life and institutions of the invaders, such as cnear, ‘war-ship’; fylcian, ‘to collect’; ōra, the name of a coin; and so on. Those words and expressions which appear at a later date, on the other hand, reveal something very different from the superficial relations between the two peoples, such as the above words point to. The later words include several adverbs, pronouns, and other words which show a close and intimate connection between English and Scandinavian speakers.

The fact that practically no prose literature of the early period has survived in any but a West Saxon form no doubt also accounts to a certain extent for the paucity of Scandinavian words actually recorded in O.E. itself. The list of these words given by Kluge, Paul’s Grundr.², p. 932, etc., includes many words whose Scandinavian origin is doubtful. The close affinity of sounds and vocabulary between the two languages makes it in many cases practically impossible to be certain whether the word in question is really a Norse loan-word or an original English word. The question of the linguistic tests of true Scandinavian words will fall to be discussed in the next chapter.


CHAPTER XIII

THE MIDDLE ENGLISH PERIOD

A complete account of the various forms of English speech, which should trace the development of each and show their mutual relations, would be a most complicated task, and one which in the present state of knowledge would be impossible.

The difficulty arises partly in the number of M.E. texts, and the great dialectal variety which they display; partly also in the fact that the remains of O.E. outside the West Saxon dialect are so scanty.

The modern dialects are not, as a rule, the representatives of the M.E. dialects, except in certain of their most pronounced features, such as the Northern (ē or ī, etc.), as contrasted with South and Midland (ou), which both represent Common O.E. ā. Most of the peculiarities of the modern dialects are of quite recent development, and afford but little help in elucidating the problems of the M.E. period. It is quite possible, of course, that many features of the present-day dialects, which it is impossible to discover from the texts of the earlier period, may already have been developed, but could find no adequate expression in the spelling. On the other hand, there is no doubt whatever that the majority of the most characteristic features of Middle Kentish and Middle DIALECTAL DIVERSITY IN MIDDLE ENGLISH Southern (from Somersetshire to Sussex) have completely vanished from the modern speech of those areas. The Middle English dialects, therefore, stand to a great extent isolated; of some, we cannot watch the early development, owing to the loss or absence of records of the oldest period; while there are others whose subsequent career we cannot trace, because they have perished.

Towards the end of the fourteenth century there emerges, from among the many provincial forms which had hitherto been used for literary purposes, a dialect, chiefly Midland in character, but containing some elements at least of all the other chief dialectal types, which henceforth serves as the exclusive form of speech used in literature, and from which Modern Standard English is descended. This, with certain variations, is the English of Chaucer, of Wycliff, and of Gower.

The precise area in which the literary dialect arose is still disputed, but there can be little doubt that, whatever may have been its precise antecedents, it was a real living form of speech, not a literary concoction, and that the English of Chaucer is the flexible, racy speech of a class, if not of a province, most probably that of the upper strata of English educated society—the language at once of the nobles and officials of the Court, and of the scholars and divines of the University of Oxford.

It is true that in a few cases the Modern Standard English form of a given word cannot be traced directly to that particular M.E. type which is found in Chaucer’s language; but, speaking generally, we may say that the literary English of to-day is the lineal representative of the dialect in which Chaucer writes. This being the case, the most practical course for the student of the history of the English language is to consider M.E. as culminating in the dialect of literature as found in Chaucer, and to take that as the M.E. type from which he traces Modern English.

But in order to understand, even approximately, the development of Chaucer’s English from the older forms, the beginner must become acquainted with the chief general M.E. characteristics, of sound change, inflexional system, and vocabulary.

He must, further, consider the main characteristic features of the principal M.E. dialectal types, in order that he may recognise their forms in Chaucer’s language and in that of the modern period.

General Authorities on the Middle English Period.

So far there is no complete and minute M.E. Grammar, and we have largely to rely upon monographs of particular texts. The principal M.E. Grammar is that of Morsbach, Mittelenglische Grammatik, 1 Theil, Halle, 1896. This is minute, and deals with the phonology of all the dialects. So far as it goes, this is a most valuable book for the advanced student, but, unfortunately, it breaks off in the middle of a paragraph, without having dealt with the whole vowel system. In this work the texts and authorities of each dialect are enumerated, and the problems of accent and quantity are exhaustively treated, In the second volume of Kaluza’s Historische Grammatik der Englischen Sprache, Berlin, 1901, the main features of M.E. are dealt with in a short space, and in a manner which is practical and convenient for beginners, especially CHRONOLOGICAL AND DIALECTAL DIVISIONS those whose main object is to trace the history of the standard language. Sound and suggestive, though difficult to use on account of lack of systematic arrangement, is Kluge’s Geschichte d. Engl. Spr. in Paul’s Grundriss. The development of M.E. sounds from O.E. is dealt with in Sweet’s History of English Sounds (H.E.S.), Oxford, 1888, pp. 154-198; and the same writer’s New English Grammar, Part 1., Oxford, 1892, Shorter English Historical Grammar, and Primer of Historical English Grammar (the latter a masterpiece of concise and accurate statement), all give a short but clear account of the main characteristics of M.E. in their relation both to the earlier and the later forms of English. An exceedingly useful sketch of M.E. Grammar for beginners is also prefixed to Specimens of Early English—Part I., from 1150-1300; Part II., 1298-1393.

Other general works and monographs dealing with specific texts will be referred to in the course of this chapter.

Chronological Divisions of Middle English.

We may adopt Sweet’s divisions, which are: Transition O.E., 1100-1200; Early M.E., 1200-1300; Late M.E., 1330-1400.

Dialectal Divisions of Middle English.

It is possible to distinguish four chief dialectal types, which correspond to the O.E. divisions, although within each of the original dialectal areas numerous sub-varieties are recorded in M.E. The principal dialect groups are:

(1) Northern, descended from Old Northumbrian. By the beginning of the fourteenth century it is possible to distinguish between Scots and Northern English, although the former name (M.E. Scotis) appears to have been applied only to Gaelic speech down to the sixteenth century.

(2) Midland, which corresponds to the old dialects of Mercia and East Anglia. The Midland area reaches as far south as the Thames.

(3) The Southern, or Saxon Dialects; and

(4) The Dialect of Kent.

Texts representing the Chief Dialects.

It will be unnecessary here to do more than enumerate a few of the chief M.E. texts, of which the date of the manuscript and the place in which it was written is well established.

A. Transition TextsEast Midland.A.S. Chronicle, Laud MS., from 1122-1154, probably written about 1154 at Peterborough. Extracts from this are to be found in Skeat’s Specimens, Part I. The whole text may be read either in Thorpe’s Ed. of A.S. Chronicle (Rolls Series) or in Plummer’s Two Saxon Chronicles, Oxford, 1892.

Southern.History of the Holy Rood-tree, circa 1170, Ed. Napier, E.E.T.S., 1894.

B. Early Middle EnglishNorthern.Metrical Psalter, Yorkshire, before 1300. Extracts in Specimens, Part II., Ed. Surtees Soc., 1843-1847; Cursor Mundi, circa 1300; Specimens, Part II.

Midland.The Ormulum, written in Lincolnshire in 1200. Extracts occur in Sweet’s First Middle English Primer and in Skeat’s Specimens. The most recent complete edition is that of Holt, Oxford, 1878.

Southern.Ancren Riwle (A.R.), Dorsetshire, circa 1225. REPRESENTATIVE MIDDLE ENGLISH TEXTS Extracts in Sweet’s Middle English Primer and the Specimens. In the latter book other Dorsetshire texts of about the same period, and perhaps by the same author, may be studied. The standard edition of A.R. is that of Morton, Camden Soc., 1852.

Kentish.—Various Sermons and Homilies in the Kentish Dialect, from 1200-1250, are to be found in Skeat’s Specimens, Part I.

C. Late Middle EnglishNorthern.Prick of Conscience (Hampole), Yorks, before 1349; Specimens, Part II., Ed. Morris, E.E.T.S.

Midland.Alliterative Poems, Lancashire, circa 1360; Specimens, Ed. Morris, E.E.T.S., 1869; Earliest Prose Psalter, West Midland, 1375, Ed. Bülbring, E.E.T.S., 1891.

Southern.St. Editha, Wilts, 1400, Ed. Horstmann, 1883.

Kentish.Ayenbite of Inwyt, 1340; see Specimens, Part II., Ed. Morris, E.E.T.S., 1866.

We have, unfortunately, no Northern texts of this period earlier than the two mentioned in A above—that is to say, nothing to bridge the gulf of more than two hundred years, and no texts produced in Scotland till the Bruce, 1375.

General Characteristics of Middle English compared with Old English.

A. Middle English Orthography.—The changes in spelling which distinguish the period with which we are dealing with that which went before are of a twofold nature. There are, firstly, the changes introduced in an attempt to express the changes which were taking place in pronunciation; and, secondly, those due to the application of an entirely different system of sound notation, which was in the main Norman French. The former class will be more fully treated in enumerating the M.E. sound changes.

The influence of French spelling is present in various degrees even in very early M.E. texts, and even before the Conquest. Thus u, instead of the English intervocalic f to express a voiced sound, occurs in an eleventh-century manuscript. Later on u is universal in such a Southern text as A.R., although Northern texts retain f much later even in French words. The Midland Orm writes serrfenn usually, but serruen only once (H.E.S., 602).

The spelling of the Ormulum, which is so remarkably consistent and methodical as to call for special notice, shows only very slight touches of Norman influence, but is partly the English traditional spelling, with modifications introduced by the writer Orm for purposes of greater phonetic exactitude.

As the knowledge of French and French documents became more and more widespread among educated Englishmen, the French mode of expressing sounds became fixed, so that, instead of the orthography being English, slightly influenced by French, as in the case of some early M.E. manuscripts, that of the late M.E. period is principally basally French, with a certain residue of traditional English spellings.

In the South, where we find the largest proportion of Anglo-French loan-words in the early period, French orthography begins earlier than in the North and Midlands. French loan-words retain their regular French CHANGES IN SPELLING spelling, and this system is then transferred to English words containing sounds approximately the same as those occurring in French. Thus already in A.R. we find French c (= s) transferred to English words, as in seldcēne, ‘seldom-seen.’

The following is a list of some of the chief novelties in M.E. spelling; many of them have survived in the English spelling of the present day:

Vowels.o written for O.E. u in the neighbourhood of n, m, v, w; a purely graphic attempt to distinguish letters which resemble each other in shape: sone, ‘son,’ O.E. sunu. The sound itself (u) remains during the M.E. period.

u written for O.E. y when this sound is preserved, otherwise for A.-Fr. u, which had the sound of y (i.e., high-front-round); cf. wurchen, O.E. wyrċan. When long, the same sound is written ui (in the South), to represent O.E. ȳ; huiren, ‘hear,’ O.E. hȳran.

ou for O.E. ū, and for A.-Fr. (ū)-sound: hous, ‘house,’ O.E. hūs; court. This spelling is very rare for the short (u)-sound.

ie occurs in Gower and other texts to express a long tense (ē), as distinct from the slack (ϵ̄), written e: hieren. ‘hear,’ O.E. (non-W.S.), hēran.

y is written for (ī). It never expresses the rounded (y) in M.E.

Consonants.ch is written for O.E. ċ already in the middle of the twelfth century (cf. the so-called Kentish Gospels, for instance): chester, O.E. (Kentish, etc.) ċester; chēke, O.E. cēāe, ‘cheek.’ Medially cch or chch occur. -tch- is rare before the fifteenth century.

gg is written for the O.E. ċġ: brigge, brugge, O.E. bryċġ, ‘bridge.’ The spelling -dg- for this sound is not common before the fifteenth century.

j is written initially for the same sound, which only occurs in this position in French words: jugement, etc.

The O.E. symbol ʓ, slightly modified in shape, is retained in M.E. to express the front-open voiced consonant: ȝiuen, ‘give,’ O.E. ġiefan; weȝ̇, ‘way,’ O.E. weġ. The use of y for this sound belongs to the later M.E. period.

The symbol g is a new symbol imported by French scribes. Prior to the Conquest, ʓ was the only form of the letter, and did duty for both back and front consonants. The new symbol appears first about the first quarter of the twelfth century. At first the scribes use the English symbol ʓ and the Continental g indiscriminately for either the back or the front sound. From the thirteenth century onwards, however, the distinction is usually consistently made, the modified form ȝ of the old letter ʓ being used for the latter, the new for the former sound. Orm makes the distinction most carefully, and further introduces a symbol of his own, a combination of the Continental g and English ʓ, to express a back stop, in words like ȳod, etc.

[Note.—This interesting and important discovery was made by Professor Napier. Cf. Academy, 1890, p. 188, and the reprint of the article in History of the Holy Rood-tree, E.E.T.S., 1894, p. 71.]

gu, the French symbol for a back stop before front vowels, is still retained in guest. In M.E. it is sometimes written in guod, ‘good,’ and kingue.

gh is written for a back-open voiceless consonant, O.E. h: inogh, ‘enough,’ O.E. ġenōh.

sch, ssch, sh, are written for O.E. , and less commonly CHANGES IN PRONUNCIATION ss and s: schip, ssip, flessch, fless, etc.

th replaces þ and ð: thinken, etc., in Late M.E.

qu replaces O.E. cw: quēne, ‘woman’ (kwϵ̄ne), O.E. cwĕne; queen, ‘queen’ (kwēn), O.E. cwēn.

c is used for (s) in French words, as at present in face, etc., and occasionally, as we have seen, in English words as well.

u, and later v, are used medially, instead of O.E. f, to express the voiced sound: lauerd, O.E. hlāford, ‘lord’; euel and evel, ‘evil,’ O.E. (Kentish) efel. In Southern texts, where O.E. f was voiced initially, u, v are written in that position: uorþ, O.E. forþ. In A.R. f is still written finally, to avoid confusion with the vowel, as in līf, ‘life’; also before voiced consonants, as in hefde, ‘had,’ O.E. hœfde.

B. Middle English Sounds.—The quality of M.E. sounds is established partly from historical considerations of their origin and subsequent development, partly from the various phonetic attempts to render them made by the scribes, partly by the rhymes of the M.E. period.

By the last means we are able, for instance, to show the existence of two long ‘e’-sounds, although the M.E. spelling does not in all cases distinguish. Chaucer, a careful and accomplished maker of rhymes, never rhymes M.E. ē, the result of a M.E. lengthening of O.E. ĕ, as in bēren, O.E. bĕran, with the other ē inherited from O.E., as in hēren, ‘hear,’ O.E. hēran. Further, we still distinguish between the sounds of the two words ‘hear’ and ‘bear.’ There can be little doubt that in M.E. the sound in hēren was a mid-front-tense, whereas that in ‘bēren was mid-front-slack. This M.E. distinction is still further confirmed by the scribal distinction, already noted, of ie for the former class of words, and e for the latter.

The quantity of vowels is established by the means just described, which are, however, even more conclusive in settling the quantity than they are in determining the precise quality of a vowel.

For the quantities of early M.E. the Ormuium is invaluable, since the writer invariably doubles the consonant after short vowels, or, in the few cases where this is not practicable, marks the short quantity thus: năme, ‘name,’ etc.

We may assume that when Orm does not double the consonant, the preceding vowel is long. Thus he distinguishes between the singular lamb, with long ā, already in O.E., and the plural lammbre, where the combination of consonants (mbr) has prevented lengthening.

Marks to show that a vowel is long are rare in M.E., but the doubling of vowels for this purpose, although not consistently practised in early M.E., is very common, and fairly regularly carried out in later M.E., as in Chaucer’s stoon, ‘stone’; heeth, ‘heath,’ etc.

Qualitative Sound Changes in Middle English.

1. O.E. ā, which includes both original ā and ā lengthened from ă during the O.E. period, before -ld, -mb, -nd, hānd, lāmb, and Anglian āld (M.E. lōmb, hōnd, ōld), is, rounded to ō (ɔ̄) in the South and Midlands: O.E. hām, ‘home,’ M.E. hōm; O.E. sār, ‘sore,’ M.E. sōr, etc.

In the North, except before l + another consonant, ā is gradually fronted to ē through intermediate stage O.E. A ROUNDED IN SOUTH, FRONTED IN NORTH of œ̄. This sound is written a in the North of England, but in Scotland often ai. Its front character can be shown from the M.E. rhymes, and also from the Mod. Scots and Northern Eng. dialect forms, which show (ē, īə), etc.

The Southern and Midland rounding must have begun very early, since no N.-Fr. word with ā, such as dāme, ‘lady,’ fāme, etc., ever shows any trace of the process. Therefore, before the period of the earliest loan-words from Norman sources, O.E. ā and Fr. ā were already distinct. The early manuscripts are by no means consistent in writing o for the old ā sound. The Kentish Homilies (MS. Vespas., A. 22, before 1150) occasionally writes ō by the side of the usual ā. The Laud MS. of the Chronicle has one example, mōre, under the year 1137 (cf. Skeat’s Specimens, I., p. 11, l. 42). This manuscript was probably written after the year 1154. Orm (1200), though such a careful orthographist, writes a in all cases, never o. This probably indicates that the change had not gone far enough in his dialect, to be recognisable as a new sound. Genesis and Exodus, also E. Midl. fifty years later, has plenty of ō spellings. The so-called Lambeth Homilies (before 1200) has no ō, but always ā; while the collection of Homilies of the same date in Trinity College, Cambridge, have ō universally, and apparently no ā’s. Ancren Riwle (1225) has ō, oa in hundreds of cases, a occurring only once in an unequivocal word, wāt; lātes, from O.N. lāt, lœ̄te, is thus written five times. [On this text, cf. Ostermann, Bonner Beitr., 1905.] It is therefore clear that the rounding of ā had been carried out in the South and in some Midland dialects by the second half of the twelfth century, even although the scribes do not consistently express this in their spellings. On the other hand, it can be proved by an examination of the rhymes of Barbour’s Bruce (1375) that by that date the Northern fronting was fully complete. ansuḗrmar, O.E. māra, ‘more’ (Book I., 437, 438); war, ‘was,’ O.E. (Northern) wēron, rhymes to mar (Book II., 59, 60); war to rair, ‘roar,’ O.E. rāran (Book IV., 422, 423). The front quality of the vowel in war, in spite of the spelling, is proved by the rhyme of wer, with different spelling, to French manér (Book IV., 7, 8), and by that of ere, O.E. œ̄r, to were (Book IV., 402, 403). The vowel in all these words is certainly front, either (ǣ) or (ɛ̄), or even possibly (ē), which is suggested by the rhyme neir, ‘near,’ manéir (Book IV., 377, 378). In the sixteenth century the rhyme drēme, ‘dream,’ O.E. drēām, with hāme, is noted by Professor Gregory Smith in Specimens of Middle Scots, p. xx; cf. also ibid., p. 174, lines 13, 14, in a poem by Sir David Lindsay.

2. O.E. œ̄ (1), when original, was very early in the O.E. period raised to ē in all dialects but W. Saxon. This sound is represented in the earliest M.E. (Southern) texts by the spellings œ or ea, the levelling of œ̄ with the old long diphthong having already taken place in O.E. Later on this sound seems to disappear altogether, even in Southern, the non-Saxon ē penetrating from the other dialects.

O.E. œ̄ (2), which was the i-mutation of ā, survives, in all dialects but Kentish, throughout the O.E. period. In M.E. it was gradually raised to (ɛ̄), written œ, ea, ee. In Mod. Eng. this sound, in common with Anglian ē, has TREATMENT OF Ō IN NORTHERN DIALECTS become (ī), but its origin is often expressed by the spelling ea, as in heath, O.E. hœ̄þ, from *hāþi, as distinguished from deed, from non-W.S. dēd, earlier dœ̄d, with original œ. This M.E. (ɛ̄) was not raised to (ī) in Mod. Eng. until much later than the M.E. tense sound, and is still preserved as (ɛ̄), etc., in Irish English (cf. pp. 320, 321).

3. O.E. ō, often written oo in M.E., was pronounced with increased rounding, and by the period of Chaucer had probably reached a sound closely resembling Swedish ō, which to the ear is almost like ū. In the sixteenth century the full (ū) sound was developed. In the North O.E. ō had a different development, as is shown by such rhymes in Northern Eng. and Scotch texts as fortṓnesōne, ‘soon’ (Pricke of Consc., 1273-1274, circa 1340); auentūreforfūre, ‘perished,’ O.E. forfōr (Bruce, Book X., 528, 529); blūdrūde (Schir W. Wallace, 1488, Book II., 91, 92). In the same poem, Book II., we find fūde, ‘food,’ O.E. fōda (308), blūd (311), gūd (312), all rhyming with conclūd (314). There are numerous examples of such rhymes in Scotch texts. Here we find, then, O.E. ō written o, u, oi, etc., rhyming with French ū (ȳ), which is also spelled in exactly the same ways as the former sound. The inference is that in Northern Eng. and Scotch, by the fourteenth century, at any rate, the two sounds were felt as identical. Whatever may have been the precise sound intended, it is clear that its acoustic effect was approximately that of a high-front-round vowel, or perhaps a high-mixed-round, that it was the ancestor of the various sounds representing O.E. ō, which we find in the modern dialects of Scotland and the North of England, and that it evidently did not pass through the (ū) stage which is universal in the South and Midlands.

4. O.E. ȳ is unrounded everywhere but in the South to ī, which shares the same development as original ī, and becomes (ai) in Mod. Eng. In the South the ȳ sound is preserved, and is written u or ui. The Southern forms have died out, with the exception of ‘bruise’ (brūz), O.E. brȳsan, which has preserved the characteristic M.E. Sthn. spelling. It must be noted that ȳ became ē in Kentish already in the middle of the ninth century, and this sound, together with all other O.E. ē’s, is preserved in M.E. in that dialect.

5. O.E. ē, ī, and ū were preserved unaltered, unless affected by a M.E. process of shortening (see p. 270, etc.), so far as the evidence goes, during the whole M.E. period, (ē) was raised to (ī) in the early Modern period; ū was diphthongized in the South and Midlands about the same time, to a sound which subsequently became (au). The Norman spelling ou to express ū has been retained, and is now popularly regarded as the natural symbol of the modern diphthong. (ī) was diphthongized to (əi) in the sixteenth century, and from it (ai) has developed, with slight variations, in all dialects.

The Short Vowels.—With the exception of O.E. œ̆, these undergo no qualitative change during the M.E. period.

6. O.E. œ̆ appears already in O.E., as e in Kentish, and to a certain extent in Mercian. In W. Sax. and Northumbrian œ is preserved. In M.E., Southern texts, especially Kentish, preserve e, but otherwise a is the usual form. Chaucer has fader, ‘father,’ O.E. fœder; water, O.E. wœter, ‘water.’

In the later language the e-forms disappear altogether. LOSS OF OLD DIPHTHONGS IN M.E. In combination with ȝ̇, e forms in Kentish a diphthong, written ei.

Those dialects which have a combine this sound into the diphthong ai with the following ȝ̇, as in dai. Sometimes i, sometimes ȝ̇ is written. In early texts the O.E. distinction between the sing. and pl. of such words as dœġ, pl. dagas, etc., is preserved: dai, dawes, etc. (on change of O.E. g to w, see p. 274 below). Chaucer has dai, day, dayes, etc., with the ȝ̇ of the sing. generalized throughout. On the other hand, he has the vb. dawen, ‘dawn,’ from O.E. dagian, earlier *dagōjan. Apparently, the diphthongs ei ai were scarcely distinguishable in M.E. The vowel in wei, ‘way,’ rein, ‘rain,’ O.E. weġ, reġn, has had precisely the same development as that in dai, O.E. dœġ, and wain, O.E. wœġn, ‘wain.’

O.E. a when preserved, is, of course, indistinguishable from œ in M.E.

The O.E. Diphthongs.—Such of these as survive the various O.E. combinative factors in the different dialects, which tend to monophthongize them, are completely monophthongized in the M.E. period, except in Kentish, where the spellings dyath, ‘death,’ O.E. de̅a̅þ, þyef, ‘thief,’ O.E. þe̅o̅f, seem to imply a diphthongal pronunciation. But with the dying out of the Kentish dialect all trace of the original diphthongs, as such, disappears.

Otherwise, O.E. e̅a̅ is monophthongized to (ǣ) in early M.E., and e̅o̅ to (ē). The diphthongal spellings, are, however, common in early texts, in spite of the undoubted change of sound. Similarly, the short diphthongs ea and eo become (æ) and (e) respectively. This is proved by the fact that ēā, ēō are not infrequently written for old œ̄, ē, and conversely; while the original short œ and e are often expressed by ea and eo respectively. In fact, in early texts e̅͝a̅ is a regular symbol for, and proves the existence of, the sounds (ǣ̆). This (ǣ̆), representing the original diphthongs, was, together with original ǣ̆, raised to (ɛ̄̆). The new (ē) sound was completely levelled under original O.E. ē, and the original O.E. ĕ, when preserved short, was levelled under the new ĕ.

Mod. Eng. weald, side by side with wold, appears to represent the Saxon weald, E.M.E. wœ̄ld, whence wēld (ɛ̄). Early Mod. (wēld). Wold is, of course, the old Anglian wāld. The early Middle Kentish chold, ‘cold,’ is apparently a mixture of Southern ċœld, chœld, and Anglian cāld, cōld.

The Development of New Diphthongs in Middle English.

The various diphthongs which came into existence during the M.E. period are the result either of the vocalizing of O.E. ġ (front-open voice consonant) after a preceding œ or e, as has been already indicated above, as in dei, dai, rein, etc.; of the development of a front vowel glide before fronted h, as in heih, ‘high,’ O. Angl. hēh, etc.; or the development of a back vowel glide between a back vowel and a back-open consonant, as in douhter, O.E. dohter; inōuh, ‘enough,’ O.E. ġenōh, plōuh, ‘plough,’ O.E. plōh. In late O.E. the last two words become inūh and plūh respectively, by the over-rounding and raising of (ō) to (ū) through the influence of the second element of the diphthong, and the subsequent contraction of (ūu) to (ū). The literary English (plau) and the archaic (inau) ‘enow’ are the result, not of the old nom., which in Late O.E. had h, but of the oblique cases, where the voice sound was retained—O.E. genōge, plōges. This O.E. g became w in M.E.—inōwe, plōwes, etc., where ōu or ōw had the same sound as in the Nom. The sometime existence of the actual diphthong (ōu) is confirmed by the Modern dialect form (plōh), in which the second element has been lost. The standard English (inaf), ‘enough,’ represents the old nom.; and so do the dialect forms (plūh, plūf, inūh), etc. The O.E. combination ag- before vowels produces M.E. aw-au (cf. O.E. dragan, M.E. drawen).

In O.E. af- the consonant is sometimes weakened to a vowel, thus forming the second element of a diphthong—O.E. hafoc, M.E. hauk; and the same thing may happen to O.E. ef-, as in M.E. eute, ‘newt,’ O.E. efete.

The combination au- in Norman French words was pronounced (aun) by some speakers, presumably in imitation of the original nasal vowel. Such spellings as daungerous, aungel, ‘angel,’ are frequent, and they survive in many cases in Mod. Eng.—e.g., haunt, haunch, aunt, jaundice, laundry, etc. Here the fluctuation of the Mod. Eng. pronunciation between (ɔ̄) and (ā) makes it evident that two types, one (au) and the other (aun), existed in M.E. The Mod. Eng. (hɔ̄ntʃ, džɔ̄ndis, lɔ̄ndri), etc., go back to M.E. (hauntʃ, džaundis), etc.; while the Mod. Eng. pronunciations (hāntʃ, džāndis, ānt), etc., are descended from M.E. forms without diphthongization. In the same way Mod. Eng. al-, pronounced (ɔ̄l-), also presupposes an earlier (aul-), as in Mod. Eng. (ɔ̄l, sɔ̄lt, bɔ̄l) = ‘all,’ ‘salt,’ ‘bawl,’ from (aul, sault, baul). This is apparently the result of the development of a parasitic (u) between a and the following l.

Quantitative Vowel Changes in Middle English.

1. Lengthening of Original Short Vowels.

(a) Early Lengthening before Consonantal Combinations.—As we have seen, all short vowels were lengthened in late O.E. before certain consonantal combinations. Unless conditions arise to shorten these vowels again, their length is preserved in M.E. In the case of the lengthened a before -ld, mb, nd, ng, the survival of the new quantity is made certain by the spellings hōnd (Orm hānd), strōng (Orm strang), etc., which show that the lengthened ā is rounded to ō together with original O.E. ā, in hām, M.E. hōm, etc. In other cases we have to depend upon Orm’s spellings (ante, p. 260), the occasional marks of length in the manuscripts, rhymes of the new long vowels with original longs, and the later history of the words in English. Thus from the latter point of view Mod. Eng. find (faind) field (fīld), hound (haund), can only be derived from M.E. types with the long vowels ī, ē, and ū respectively. Orm’s spellings, findenn, feld, hund, corroborate the assumption of the existence of such types, as do the other M.E. spellings, field (ē), hound (ū), which have survived to the present day.

In certain words, such as hand, lamb, etc., where we should expect a M.E. lengthening, on account of the presence of the combinations -mb, -nd, etc., the Mod. Eng. forms nevertheless presuppose M.E. forms with a short vowel. In these cases we must assume that both long and short forms existed in M.E., the latter types produced VOWEL-LENGTH IN OPEN SYLLABLES by inflexion. (On this point see pp. 271-273 below.)

(b) Later Lengthening of Vowels in an Open Syllable.—By the first half of the thirteenth century, the typical M.E. lengthening of the vowel a, œ, e, o in open syllables was complete, and had taken place in all dialects.

This is shown by the frequent rhyming of original short vowels in this position, with original longs: swēteeðgēte, O.E. swēte, e̅a̅ðġĕte; ōrevorlōre(n), O.E. ār, forlŏren [cf. Morsbach, M.E. Gr., p. 86]. Such rhymes at least prove agreement in quantity, if not in the quality of the vowels.

Again, already in Orm we find faderr, ‘father,’ O.E. fœ̆der, and waterr, O.E. wœ̆ter, with (ā); etenn, ‘eat,’ O.E. ĕtan; chele, ‘cold,’ O.E. (non-W.S.) ċĕle, both with (ɛ̄); chosenn, p.p. of chēsenn, ‘choose,’ O.E. cĕren, (Orm’s p.p. has s on the analogy of the inf. and pres. indic.); hope, O.E. hŏpu, both with (ɔ̄). The Mod. Eng. spelling ‘eat’ implies a long slack (ɛ̄)—at any rate down to the sixteenth century, when the corresponding tense sound was written ee, and was raised to (ī). The lengthened ō must also have had a different sound in M.E. from the original ō. The latter became (ū) in the sixteenth century; the latter was still (ɔ̄), and was later, in the seventeenth century, raised to (ō). (See below, pp. 323, 324, on development of the two ō-sounds in the sixteenth and seventeenth centuries.) The sounds in Mod. Eng. water and father (ɔ̄ and ā) do not represent the normal independent development of this M.E. ā. The vowel in water is influenced by the w, and that in father is from a M.E. doublet with a short vowel. (See below, pp. 271 and 317.)

M.E. ā, whether due to lengthening of older ă, or whether it be a N. Fr. ā, develops in standard Mod. Eng. into the diphthong (ɛi), with the same sound as the name of the first letter of the alphabet. Thus O.E. năma, M.E. nāme, Mod. Eng. (nɛim); N. Fr. dāme, Mod. Eng. dɛim. The dialectal (fɛiðər or fēðər) exactly represent M.E. fāder, so far as the long vowel is concerned.

2. Vowel Shortening in Middle English.

The chief factor of vowel shortening in M.E. is the presence of a long or double consonant, or a group of consonants, immediately after the vowel.

From the above statement, those consonant groups which, as we have seen (ante, p. 235), tend to lengthen a short vowel, must, of course, be excepted.

It is immaterial whether the shortening group occurs in the body of a simple word or arises in composition, provided that the combination existed before the shortening process began. Examples:

A. Before double consonants:

1. Mette, ‘met,’ O.E. mētte, from *mēt-de, from mētede.

B. Before other consonant groups:

1. Two stops: keppte, ‘kept,’ O.E. cēpte; sleppte, ‘slept,’ O.E. slēpte.

2. Stop + divided, or nasal: ŭtmōst, O.E. ūtmest; little, O.E. lȳtle; chappmenn, O.E. ċēāpmenn.

3. Stop + open cons.: dĕpthe, O.E. *dēpþu or *dēopþu; Ĕdward, O.E. Eādward.

4. Open cons. + stop: soffte, ‘soft,’ O.E. sōfte; wissdōm, O.E. wīsdōm; sohhte, O.E. sōhte, ‘sought.’

5. Open cons. + divided or nasal cons.: gŏsling, dimin. SHORTENING OF OLD LONG VOWELS of gōs; deffles, ‘devils,’ O.E. de̅o̅fol; wimman, from wīfmann.

6. Open cons. + open cons. or h: hŭswīf, Mod. Eng. (hazif); gŏshauk, O.E. gōshafoc.

7. Nasal cons. + stop: flemmde, ‘put to flight,’ O.E. (Angl.) flēmde.

8. Divided or nasal cons. + open cons.: hallghenn, ‘hallow,’ later M.E. hălwen; fillthe, ‘filth,’ O.E. fȳlþ; mŏnthe, ‘month,’ O.E. mōnaþ; obl. cases, mōnþe, etc.

9. Nasal + divided cons.: clennlike, O.E. clǣnlīċe.

[Note.—The words with doubled consonants above are Orm’s spelling, which proves the preceding vowels to be short.]

It will be observed that under the conditions enumerated not only are original O.E. long vowels shortened, but also that the new (M.E.) long vowels, developed in open syllables, do not arise here, in close syllables.

The occurrence in the declension, conjugation, or other inflection of a word of both open and close syllables is of great importance for the subsequent history of the language. In this way doublets arose of the same word, one with a long, the other with a short. Thus the nouns fāder and wāter were long, but in the inflected forms the combinations -dr-, -tr- arose by the syncope of the e of the second syllable. The genitives were fădres, wătres. Similarly, words which had original long vowels underwent shortening in inflection as a result of syncope. Thus dēvel in nom. form, O.E. dēōfol, had pl. dĕvles (cf. Orm’s dĕffles above); from this shortened type, which gave rise to a new nom., Mod. Eng. (dɛvil) is derived.

Shortening was apparently normal before -st and -sch (ʃ), O.E . Words with original long vowels before these combinations show, however, some fluctuation of quantity in M.E. Thus O.E. brēost became M.E. brēst, whence brĕst. Brēst, however, is also found, and this type is probably due to the inflected forms, where the syllable division was brē-stess, etc. Modern dialect forms, such as (brīst, brēst), also exist (cf. also ‘priest,’ M.E. prē-stes). In the same way Standard Mod. Eng. flesh goes back to a type (fleʃ) in M.E. But the M.E. form with the long vowel (Orm has flœ̄sh) must be due to the syllable division of Gen. flœ̄-shes, etc.

The Late O.E. lengthenings before -nd, -mb, etc., are also liable to show short forms in Standard Mod. Eng. In many cases here, too, doublets arose in inflection, since the lengthening either never took place or was got rid of before a third consonant. Thus Mod. Eng. lamb, compared with M.E. lōmb, clearly goes back to a M.E. type with a short vowel, such as occurs in the plural lămbre. Mod. Eng. hand (hænd) perhaps arose from such compounds as handful. Mod. Eng. friend (frɛnd), by the side of M.E. frēnd, from O.E. fre̅o̅nd, is from a shortened M.E. type, which arose, perhaps, in the compound frĕndschipe. The Scotch dialects preserve the representative of the long M.E. type here, as does Standard English also in fiend (fīnd), M.E fēnd, O.E. fe̅o̅nd. Mod. Eng. childchildren (tʃaid—tʃildrən) preserve the normal interchange of long and short seen in Orm’s child, pl. chilldre. There are some short forms in Mod. Eng. which it is difficult to account for, unless we assume that shortening could take place within the longer breath group or sentence under the same conditions as those which caused it in the inflected word or compound. Such are land (lænd) compared with MODERN EQUIVALENTS OF M.E. DOUBLETS M.E. lōnd, Orm lānd; and band (bænd) compared with bond. The latter represents a much later shortening of M.E. bōnd, O.E. bānd, similar to that which has taken place also in long, M.E. long; strong, M.E. strōng. Against the latter form Standard English has hang, sang (hæŋ, sæŋ), etc.

In most cases where O.E. short vowels were lengthened and O.E. longs shortened, the possibility of doublets existed from the inflectional or other conditions of M.E. In a vast number of cases, by comparing Standard English with the Modern dialects, it will be seen that both long and short forms have been perpetuated in modern speech.

The original rise of the doublets had nothing to do with dialectal idiosyncrasy, but the subsequent generalization of the long or short type, as the only form in use, depends upon the speech habit of the particular community. As we have seen, Standard English is by no means consistent in this respect, but uses now the descendant of a M.E. long, now of a short vowel.

The best general accounts of the quantitative and qualitative vowel changes in M.E. are to be found in Sweet’s H.E.S. and Morsbach’s M.E. Gr. The latter is particularly elaborate, though as regards the qualitative vowel changes it is unfortunately still awaiting completion.

The Treatment of the Old English Consonants in Middle English.

1. The Back Consonants.—O.E. g remained as a back stop initially before original back vowels and before consonants. Orm, as we have seen (p. 258), invented a special symbol to express this sound. Non-initially, O.E. g was an open voiced consonant, which in M.E. acquired considerable lip modification, together with a weakening of the back consonantal element, the tongue being lowered to a vowel position. The result is the Mod. Eng. w, in words like draw, M.E. drawen, O.E. dragan. Orm writes the O.E. symbol ȝ followed by h for this sound, implying probably that the back element still predominated in his pronunciation. Medially and finally M.E. w combined with the preceding vowel to form a diphthong.

O.E. c remained as a back stop in all positions. The O.E. cn- in cnāwan, etc., remained in the Standard pronunciation down to the sixteenth or early seventeenth century.

O.E. h, a voiceless back consonant, medially between before or after back vowels, remained as such in M.E. The same tendency to lip modify h existed as in the case of the voiced sound, the result in the case of h, however, being the development of a lip-teeth (f) sound, as in Mod. Eng. tough (taf), O.E. tōh. This is the normal development in Standard English and in many dialects.

In the Northern dialects the old back-open voiceless consonant remains to this day, as in Scotch (plūh), etc. Standard (plau) is, as we have seen, a doublet, formed from the oblique cases which had g in O.E. and w in M.E.

Before t, h also became (f) in M.E., brofte, O.E. brōhte occurs in Lagomon, while the Modern dialects have forms like broft, ‘brought’ (in Cornwall), and thoft, ‘thought,’ in Kent, Devon, and Cornwall. For other examples see Wright, Dialect Gr., § 359. The more usual development in this position, however, seems to have been either the voicing of h, in which case it formed the second element (u) of a diphthong, as in the types from which Standard BACK AND FRONT CONSONANTS English (dɔ̄tə, brɔ̄t, þɔ̄t), etc., sprang, or the preservation of the back-open voiceless consonant unchanged, as in Sc. (þoht), etc.

O.E. hw was apparently preserved as a voiceless w in the Lower Midlands and South; in the North and part of the Midlands the back element was strongly consonantal. This is expressed in Northern texts by the spelling qu, as in quāle, ‘whale,’ O.E. hwœl; quēt, ‘wheat,’ O.E. hwœ̄te, etc. The pronunciation (kw) is apparently unknown in the Modern dialects, and probably never developed.

Initially before vowels h remains in M.E. as a rule, though it is very early lost in the neuter pronoun hit, which already in Orm is itt. Modern Scotch still preserves the strong form hit, which is, indeed, the only form in the Sc. dialects.

The Front Consonants.—The O.E. front stops ċ and ċġ were fully assibilated to (tʃ) and (dž) early in the M.E. period. The methods of representing these sounds have already been described (ante, pp. 257, 258). For the former, the M.E. spelling ch, later tch, are conclusive, but for the latter the M.E. spellings gg are of doubtful significance, being also used for the stop, as in the Scand. legges, ‘legs.’ We have therefore to rely chiefly on the evidence of the Modern dialects to establish the existence of the (dž) sound in M.E. Unlike ch (tʃ), (dž), with the exception of one or two much-discussed words, never occurs initially in English words, though common in words of French origin, where it is usually written j in Mod. Eng., as in judge, joy, jest, etc.

The development of ċ and ċġ in M.E. and Mod. Eng. presents much difficulty, since in many cases where we should expect (tʃ and dž) we get instead back stops—dick by the side of ditch, flick by the side of flitch, seg by the THE MIDDLE ENGLISH PERIOD side of sedge, rig by the side of ridge, and so on.

The orthodox view is that in the North, O.E. ċ and ċġ were not as fully fronted as in the South, and that in M.E., or perhaps earlier, instead of developing into the full assibilated sounds, they were unfronted and became back stops. Thus words like seg, brig, and flick are looked upon as typically Northern forms, like sedge, bridge, flitch as normal Southern products.

Unfortunately, this theory, simple as it looks, will not bear investigation. It is true that M.E. texts and Modern dialects have, on the whole, more (-k and -g) and fewer (tʃ and dž) forms in the Northern, while the proportions are reversed in the Southern; but numerous assibilated forms actually do occur in the Northern, and many forms with back stops in the Southern, which on the ordinary theory can only be accounted for by the assumption of a system of wholesale borrowing. Some of the Southern k-forms, such as seek, compared with be-seech, are admittedly due to the second and third person singular: O.E. sēcst, sēcþ, M.E. sēkst, sēkþ in the Southern, where s and þ have unfronted ċ; others may be due to Scandinavian influence, though this cannot be invoked in the case of dialects which never had direct contact with Scandinavian speech. On the other hand, the occurrence of (tʃ and dž) forms in Northern dialects would seem to disprove the assertion that the O.E. front stops were not fully fronted in the North.

Fleck or flick, ‘flitch,’ in Somerset, Wilts, Hants, and Isle of Wight; seg, ‘sedge,’ in Gloucester, and, on the other hand, midge in Northumberland, Cumberland, Westmorland, Durham, and East Yorks; cletch, clutch, ‘brood THE PUZZLE OF THE INITIAL ‘PALATALS’ of chickens,’ in Northumberland, Durham, North Yorks, are troublesome forms to explain on the received theory. None of the attempted explanations of these facts are wholly satisfactory, but some are less so than others.

Initial k representing O.E. ċ, as in kettle, O.E. ċietel, ċetel; kirk, O.E. ċyrċe, etc., are universally supposed to be of Scandinavian origin. The k-forms are well established in M.E., though the normal English chetel, and of course chirche, etc., also occur, the former being comparatively rare. M.E. caf, ‘chaff’,’ compared with O.E. (W. Sax.) ċeaf, is explainable as due to the analogy of pl. O.E. cafu.

O.E. ġ initially offers further difficulties. Before ē̆ it normally appears written as ȝ, y, yh, etc., in M.E., without change of sound. Thus: for-ȝete(n), yete(n) ‘forget’; ȝelle(n), yelle(n), ‘yell’; ȝelpe(n); yelpe(n) ‘boast’; ȝēre, yēre, etc., ‘year,’ and so on.

Before i, ȝ̇ is often lost in M.E., and in some words the Modern Standard language and the dialects show the same loss quite regularly; thus O.E. ġif, ‘if,’ M.E. if; O.E. ġicel, M.E. ikyl, etc., Eng. ic-icle, O.E. ġiċċan, M.E. icching, icche(n), Mod. Eng. itch; also in the prefix ġe-, M.E. i-cume, ‘come,’ p.p. Mod. Eng. ‘yclept,’ hand-i-work, O.E. hand-ġe-weorc. M.E. also has ylde, ‘guild,’ ym-stōn, ‘gem,’ O.E. ġim-stān.

But M.E. has far more cases of ȝif, ȝim, etc., and, what is still more difficult to explain, many with g. The appearance of g- is equally difficult to understand whether it occur before i, where we should expect to find it lost altogether, or before ē̆, where we should expect M.E. ȝ, y, Mod. Eng. y. Here, apparently, we have the strange phenomenon of a front-open consonant becoming a back stop. The words in which this occurs in Standard English are: give, O.E. ġiefan, ġefan; gift; get, O.E. ġietan, ġetan; guest (with Norm. Fr. spelling gu-), O.E. ġiest, ġest; begin, O.E. be-ġinnan. To these may be added such Modern dialect forms as gif, ‘if,’ gilpie, ‘a young spark,’ related to O.E. ġielpan, ‘boast,’ and one or two others of more doubtful origin.

Now the back stop is established for M.E. in each of these words, since spellings with g occur, often by the side of those with ȝ or y, in texts from every part of the country, and Orm uses his new symbol for the back stop once at least, in gœ̄fen (pret. pl.). Further, the evidence of the Modern dialects shows that in all cases two, in a few three, M.E. types must have existed—one with g, one with y, one with the initial consonant lost. For instance, give, meaning ‘give way,’ ‘thaw,’ is found, apparently, in Norfolk, Surrey, Kent, and Somerset; yeave, verb, with same meaning, and yeavey, adjective, though now obsolete, existed a hundred years ago in Devon, and were still preserved even later in the English dialect of a West-Country colony in Wexford; eave, (h)eave, ‘to thaw,’ ‘grow moist,’ is found in West Somerset, Cornwall, and Dorset.

The modern forms are given here to supplement and confirm the evidence for the existence of three types in M.E. What is the explanation of the apparent triple mode of treatment of the same original sound in the same dialects? Clearly, we do not assert that we have here an ‘exception’ to the ordinary laws of sound change in English. Either the three forms arose under different conditions which we have failed to discriminate, or the ‘anomalous’ forms are due to some external influence.

As usual in cases of great difficulty, the influence of the MODERN FORMS WITH G- > O.E. ʓ̇- Scandinavian settlers has been called in to account for the forms with stops—give, etc. It is quite possible, of course, that in districts where Norse was spoken side by side with English, and where people knew both English ġiefan or ġefan, and Norse geva, English speakers might, when speaking their own language, substitute the initial consonant which they used in addressing the foreigners: this is possible, but it is not very likely to have taken place in such a common word. Moreover, the widespread distribution of the g-forms, which exist even in M.E. in all dialects, makes it impossible to account for them, in all cases, on the hypothesis of Scandinavian influence. In such a word as begin we might attribute the g to the pret. and p.p. O.E. began, begunnon, begunnen, and this is probably the right explanation of that form.

On the other hand, it is possible that in give we have a perfectly normal English development of a stop under conditions of strong stress, whereas with weak stress the open consonant remained. It is to be observed that it is only those O.E. ġ’s which represent original Gmc. g which are stopped in M.E. and the Modern dialects; those which represent Gmc. j, as in O.E. ġear, never become g, but remain as y, or disappear altogether. This may imply that O.E. ġ had two different pronunciations in O.E., according to its origin. If this were not the case, it is a strange coincidence that there should not be some examples of ġ = Gmc. j being stopped in subsequent times. This whole question is discussed at length in an article by the present writer in Otia Merseiana, vol. ii., History of O.E. ġ in the Middle and Modern English Dialects, in which examples are given of the distribution of each of the three forms in more than fifty M.E. texts and all the chief Modern dialects.


O.E. f and s were pronounced as voiced sounds in the South, especially in Kent in M.E., as is shown by the spelling uader, ‘father,’ zēchen, ‘seek.’ This pronunciation still survives in the Modern Southern dialects, and Standard English vat, O.E. fœt (cf. wine fat in New Testament), and vixen, O.E. fyxen, are isolated examples of forms from a Southern dialect.

Summary of Dialectal Differences.

We may summarize the chief characteristic differences of dialectal treatment of the O.E. vowels.

{In Midland, Southern, and Kentish is rounded to ō (ɔ̄) written o, oo, oa.
O.E. ā{In Northern is gradually fronted to (œ̄, ɛ̄, ē), written a, ai.
{In Northern, before l + cons., ā is diphthongized to au, which becomes ɔ̄ in Modern period.
  
O.E. œ̄¹{Becomes ē already in O.E. period in the Anglian dialects and Kentish.
(Pr. O.E. œ̄){This ē remains in M.E.
{Is preserved during O.E. period, and in M.E. in Saxon dialects; this œ̄ becomes (ɛ̄).
  
O.E. œ̄²{Preserved in all old dialects except Kentish; becomes ē there, and is retained in M.E.
i-mutation of ā{In all dialects of M.E., except Kentish, becomes (ɛ̄).
  
{In Midland, Southern, and Kentish is gradually over-rounded and raised towards (ū).
O.E. ō{In Northern is fronted or ‘mixed’, and rhymes in M.E. with French ū (= ȳ).
{This sound is written u, ui, oi, in Northern and Sc.
  
O.E. ȳ¹{Is retained only in Southern, written ui, u.
i-mutation of ū{In Northern and Midland is unrounded to ī.
{In Kentish appears as ē which had developed already in O.E. period.
  
O.E. ȳ²{The Late W. Sax. ȳ, from īē, is peculiar to this dialect; it is levelled under ȳ¹ in M.E. in Southern: huiren, ‘hear,’ Late W. Sax. hȳran.
{All the other dialects have ē already in O.E., and this remains in M.E. hēren, etc.

The Foreign Elements in Middle English.

1. (a) The Scandinavian Loan-words.—As we have LOAN WORDS IN M.E. already seen, this element appears in O.E. to a certain extent, though in that period the words from this source are chiefly those which denote things and institutions belonging to the Norsemen, and more particularly such as refer to those habits, possessions, or institutions which would naturally come under the notice of a people who were in that unfortunate relation to them in which the English continued for so long. A terrorized community who were constantly expecting the attack of rapacious pirates, in which expectation they were not disappointed, might naturally know the names which their enemies gave to their vessels—‘barda,’ ‘cnear’; and would not be unfamiliar with the name of the coins, ‘ōra,’ with which their foes may occasionally have paid for those treasures or articles of food, which were not extorted at the point of the sword. Such words as the above and others of the same nature appear, though late, in O.E. literature.

But the real influence of the Danish language upon our own was exercised when the foreigners had become permanent settlers within our country, after they had mingled their blood with our own—when they had ceased to be regarded in the light of aliens. While the amalgamation of races, through intermarriage, was taking place, there would naturally be several generations of bi-lingual speakers: persons who sprang from mixed unions between Scandinavians and English. Among such families, both tongues would be equally familiar, and when speaking English it would be an unconscious process to introduce from time to time a Norse word instead of an English one; especially as the two languages were of such close affinity that their forms were in many cases practically identical; in others, though slightly different, were yet recognisable and intelligible to English and Norse alike. To the bi-lingual period succeeded the age in which English definitely got the upper hand; the younger generations no longer spoke Norse, but the English which remained, had incorporated, and made its own, many elements from the vocabulary of the language which had died out. In some cases these loans ousted the original English words altogether.

The very closeness of the resemblance between the two languages, makes it often a matter of difficulty to determine, with absolute certainty, whether a given word is English or Norse. Björkman, in the work already quoted (ante, p. 249), points out that words could be introduced from one language into the other without either side recognising that they were foreign words. Cognate words in the two languages, which were identical in form, though slightly different in meaning, often acquired in English the sense which they possessed in Scandinavian. An example of this is O. Norse soma, ‘befit, suit,’ which is cognate with the O.E. sēman, ‘settle,’ ‘satisfy.’ In M.E. the word sēmen appears in the sense of ‘befit, suit, beseem,’ etc., which last is, of course, the modern form of the word. We may compare also the adjective seemly, M.E. sēmelich, sēmli, etc.

The phonological tests which we should naturally apply to settle the origin of a word as definitely English or Norse, are not always to be relied upon, since from the similarity of the two languages, it was possible, in adopting a word from Norse into English, to give it a thoroughly NORSE INFLUENCE ON THE LANGUAGE English form. Scandinavian words were changed to their phonological English equivalent by an unconscious etymological instinct. Thus O.E. sċ- was recognised as identical with Norse sk-, and there were a large number of words which existed in both languages, and which differed only in having sk- in one, sċ- in the other. Bi-lingual speakers who used both forms of these words could easily substitute sk- when speaking English, and might even introduce the sound into English words which had no Scandinavian equivalent. M.E. scatteren, ‘scatter,’ side by side with the genuine English form shatteren, may well be due to such a process. Again, the etymological identity of Scandinavian ei with O.E. ā was clearly perceived, and we find the Scandinavian name sveinn appearing as swān, a word which was not normally used in O.E. as a proper name, and whose Norse form is often transliterated phonetically in that language as Sweġen. Similarly, the technical term heimsōcn, ‘an attack on the house or home,’ is translated literally into O.E. as hāmsocn.

The question of the precise original affinities between Northern English and Scandinavian is obscure, on account of the absence of early records. Hence in many cases it cannot be determined with certainty which points of resemblance are due to primitive affinity, which to independent parallel development, and which to later contact.

(b) Scandinavian Suffixes in English.—Many M.E. verbs in -l- and -n- appear to be loan-words, and words with these suffixes are much more frequent in M.E. than in O.E. It seems probable that these suffixes may have spread from Scandinavian words to stems of English origin. When the suffixes occur attached to native words, doubt may exist as to whether the forms with the suffixes are wholly Scandinavian or only the suffix. Examples of -l- suffix are: M.E. babblen, ‘babble,’ Swed. babbla; M.E. bustlen, ‘wander blindly,’ O. West Scand. bustla, ‘splash about’; Mod. Eng. dialect daggle, with various meanings, such as ‘to drizzle’ and ‘to trail in the dirt,’ etc.; dangle, Swed. dialect dangla. The -n- suffix is used in Scandinavian speech to form weak intransitive verbs, generally inchoative, from verbal roots and adjectives (cf. Sweet, New English Grammar, p. 467). The -n- verbs in O.E. (cf. Sievers’ list in his As. Gr.,³ § 411, Anm. 4) are not inchoative, and are formed from adjectives or substantives which already possess an -n- suffix, such as wœcen, ‘watching,’ whence āwœcnian; fœstenian, ‘fix,’ ‘fasten,’ is from fœsten, ‘fortress,’ and so on. Examples of Scandinavian verbs with this suffix are hvītna, ‘whiten,’ i.e., ‘become white.’ Ancren Riwle has hwīten used intransitively, p. 150, l. 7 (Morton’s Ed.; cf. Skeat’s Etymological Dictionary, sub ‘whiten’), but the Metrical English Psalter, p. 50, l. 9, has ‘And over snawe sal I whitened be,’ where the word is used transitively.

Such transitive verbs as gladden, redden, frighten, etc., are new formations of M. or Mod. Eng. Most of the -n- verbs in O.E. are transitive. The intransitive usage, as well as many of the verbs themselves of this class, would appear to be of Scandinavian origin. Examples are: batten, O. Swed. batna, from root bat-, which we have in better, O.E. beter, Goth. batiz; M.E. bliknen, ‘turn pale,’ O. West Scand. blikna; M.E. dawnen, ‘dawn,’ O.E. dagian. On the other hand, O.E. costnian, M.E. costnen, ‘tempt,’ which occurs in Ælfric, is probably TRACES OF NORSE SUFFIXES native. (On the above, see also Skeat, Principles of English Etymology, i., p. 275; Kluge, Grundr.², p. 939.)

A trace of the O.N. nom. case ending -r is seen in O.E. þrœ̄ll, where the ll, which in true O.E. words, we should expect to be simplified after a long vowel, is borrowed from Norse and preserved. This long l is due to the O.N. change of -lr to ll.

The neuter suffix -t is still preserved in scant, from O.N. skamt (neuter), ‘short,’ and in M.E. wiȝt, Modern dialect wight, ‘strong,’ ‘nimble.’

In spite of the doubts that may arise in specific cases from the reasons already mentioned, the most reliable tests of the Scandinavian origin of words in English are those based upon phonological characteristics. In cases where the forms in M.E. or Mod. Eng. cannot be explained by any known law of English sound change, whereas the Scandinavian sound laws are in complete agreement with the form, we are justified, pending fresh information, in assigning a Scandinavian origin. There are, indeed, some words for which the evidence is particularly conclusive, since it can be shown that their form has been determined by prehistoric sound changes which distinguish the North Germanic, to which the Scandinavian dialects belong, from the West Germanic group, of which O.E. is a member.

A good example is the class of words which illustrate the development of Gmc. -w̄- after original short vowels. In West Gmc. this sound became a vowel, and formed a diphthong with the preceding vowel. In West Gmc., on the other hand, it was stopped to -gg(w-), and in this form remains in Scandinavian. Mod. Eng. dialect dag. ‘dew,’ also ‘to bedew,’ appears in O. West Scand. as dogg, and in N. Swed. as dagg. This represents an original *daw̄a, which regularly appears in O.E. as de̅a̅(w), M.E. deu, Mod. Eng. dew, O.H.G. tou.

Similarly, M.E. haggen, ‘cut, hew,’ represents O. West Scand. hoggua, from *haw̄an. In W. Gmc. this is regularly represented by O.E. he̅a̅wan, O.H.G. houwan, Mod. Eng. hew. Again, Mod. Eng. dialect scag, ‘to hide, take shelter,’ and scug, ‘a place of shelter,’ is from a Scandinavian skuggi, ‘shade,’ Danish skygge, ‘overshadow.’ The Gmc. form would be *skuw̄jan, *skaww̄(j)an, whence O.E. sċe̅a̅wan, German schauen. Other examples of this class of words are: egg, O. West Scand. egg, but O.E. ǣġ, M.E. ei, German ei; trig, ‘safe, tight, trim,’ etc.; O. West Scand. tryggr, ‘trusty, true,’ but O.E. tre̅o̅we, ġe-tri̅e̅we, Mod. Eng. true, O.H.G. gitriuwi, German traüe, etc.

As examples of Mod. Eng. words whose form is at variance with what must have been the fate of the genuine O.E. forms had these survived, but which may be explained on the assumption of borrowing from Scandinavian, we may take the words weak, bleak. In O.E. we have blāc, ‘pale,’ and wāk, ‘weak,’ which in Mod. Eng. must have become ‘bloke,’ ‘woke’ respectively—in fact, the M.E. ancestors of these forms blōk, wōk are actually found.

The Mod. Eng. forms, however, are clearly from O.N. bleikr, veikr. It must be admitted that the development of the vowel in the English words (ī) is not quite clear, on the assumption that they preserved the diphthong into the M.E. period, and diphthongized forms are found in M.E. On the other hand, it is possible that in some English dialects an early monopthongizing of Norse ei to (ē or ϵ̄) FRENCH INFLUENCE took place.

Another good reason which justifies us in claiming a M.E. or Mod. Eng. word as Scandinavian is the fact, if it be a common word in familiar use, that it is not found in O.E., although the usual word in Norse. Orm is particularly rich in words of this kind, and has, among many others, the following, most of which are still in use: takenn, ‘take,’ the O.E. word is niman, and ‘nim’ is still found in our dialects; til, ‘to,’ cf. un-til, and the common use of til for ‘to’ in the Northern dialects; skinn, ‘skin,’ O.E. hȳd, ‘hide’; occ., ‘and’; skill, instead of the genuine Eng. crœft; ille, instead yfel, ‘evil’; me̅o̅c, meek,’ O.N. mjūkr; gate, ‘way,’ ‘gait.’ The English pronouns they, their, them, are all of Scandinavian origin, and have entirely replaced the O.E. hie, hira, heom, of which the last two are still found in Chaucer in the form hir, hem. In addition to the authorities already quoted, see also Brate’s useful article, Nordische Lehnwörter im Ormulum, Paul and Braune’s Beitr. x.

2. The French Element.—The problems connected with the influence of French upon English during the M.E. period have been exhaustively treated by Mr. Skeat in his Principles of English Etymology, vol. ii. The student should further consult the Anhang (Supplement) on this subject, by Behrens, incorporated with Kluge’s Geschichte d. Engl. Spr. in Paul’s Grundriss, pp. 950, etc.; and Appendix III. in Mr. Bradley’s edition of Morris’s Historical Outlines of English Accidence contains a list of Norman French words from the principal English works from the twelfth to the early fourteenth century.

As the question of Norman French influence has been so thoroughly and clearly treated in the above, and is, on the whole, familiar to students of the history of English, no more need be done here than to summarize a few of the chief points of importance in this connection.

Norman French was a Northern French dialect. This dialect was spoken for about 800 years in England as a living, everyday language, at first by the official, noble, and governing classes, whose native language it was, later on by Englishmen also, even of the well-to-do sort generally. By the middle of the thirteenth century, probably, most educated persons were bi-lingual, those of Norman origin speaking at least some English, while the natives acquired the language of the foreigners. With the fusion of the races came, as we saw in the case of Norse, a fusion of vocabularies also. The Norman laws contain many technical words of English origin, while French words begin to be used in ever-increasing numbers by English writers from the year 1100 onwards.

Norman French, or, as, following Mr. Skeat, we may call it, Anglo-French, naturally had a development of its own in this country. Besides being the language of everyday life among the upper classes, this dialect was also the official dialect of the law and of Parliament down to 1362, and it continued to be taught in schools down to 1385.

With its death as an official vehicle there followed the rapid dying out of Anglo-French as a spoken language. In fact, English must have already obtained a very strong hold upon all classes before French was abolished by law as the dialect of officialdom; but the latter occurrence gave it its death-blow. We may conclude, therefore, that soon after the middle of the fourteenth century the direct source INFLECTIONS of French words of this particular origin was running low. By this time, however, hundreds of Anglo-French words had passed into the speech of Englishmen, a very large number of which have remained to this day in universal use. Chaucer’s language shows how deeply the new element had penetrated into the texture of English vocabulary; it was no longer felt as strange by his time: it was part and parcel of English.

By the side of Anglo-French words derived direct, in England itself, many others were borrowed during the fourteenth century from the French of the Continent, mostly from the Central French or Parisian dialect of the Île de France, but others also from the Picardian dialect.

The influence of Central French, both direct and through literature, which began in the M.E. period, has continued ever since, and was especially strong during the seventeenth century, as may be seen from such a comedy as Dryden’s Marriage à la Mode.

Middle English Inflections.

The changes wrought during the Transition and M.E. periods in the O.E. inflectional system are the result partly of natural sound change, partly of analogy.

As a result of the former, we may say generally that all unstressed vowels—that is, therefore, all the vowels of the endings—were levelled under ee.g., O.E. stānas, M.E. stōn-es; O.E. ēagena (gen. pl.), M.E. ēȝ̇(e)ne; O.E. wudu, M.E. wode, etc. Final m was levelled under n, which was subsequently dropped altogether.

An account of M.E. inflections is to be found in The Introduction of Morris and Skeat’s Specimens of Early English, vols. i. and ii.; and the development from O.E. is briefly traced in Sweet’s various works, already cited, upon Historical English Grammar, and in Morris’s Historical Outlines of English Accidence (Ed. Bradley).

We select here some of the leading features of the M.E. inflectional system for enumeration.

Declensions.

Substantives.—The O.E. substantives, like those in all other Gmc., or, for the matter of that, in all Aryan languages, are classified for purposes of declension, according to the nature of their stems. We distinguish vowel stems and consonantal stems. In the former case the characteristic vowel of a class followed the ‘root’ or base, and was immediately followed by the case ending: Nom. sing. Gk. λυκ-ο-ς, Gmc. *wulf-a-z, Goth. wulf-s (the stem vowel being lost in the historic period in Gmc.), O.E. wulf (with loss not only of stem vowel, but of case-ending as well); instr. pl. Lith. av-i-mis, ‘sheep,’ Goth. (dat.) gast-i-m, ‘guests,’ O.E. (dat.) sun-u-m, ‘sons.’ The stems even in Gmc. had undergone some levelling through analogy, and in O.E. all stems take the ending -um in dat. pl., the vowel in this case representing at once u and o, and the m being all that was left of the original instr. pl. case-ending -mis, fully preserved, as seen above in Lithuanian.

Consonantal stems are those which end in consonants, which sometimes, as in the case of Latin pes, ‘foot,’ from *ped-s, was the final consonant of the ‘root’ itself; in other cases, such as hom-in-em or πατ-έρ-α, was preceded by a THE RUIN OF THE DECLENSIONS vowel.

Of the consonantal stems, the most important class in O.E. is that of the -n-stems, usually known as the ‘Weak’ declension. O.E. nama, gen. sing., etc., naman, gen. pl. namna. The O.E. declensions, already greatly dilapidated by change and loss of final or other unstressed syllables, and considerably confused by analogy, as compared with that system which Comparative Philology enables scholars to reconstruct as the original Aryan, underwent further dilapidation and confusion in M.E. through the continued operation of similar factors of change. It is still possible to distinguish a-stems, u-stems, i-stems, etc., among the ‘strong’ declensions of O.E. In M.E. these are very soon all levelled under one ‘strong’ type, that of masculine a-stems. The full M.E. form of this declension runs:

Singular.Plural.
N.A.stōn.stōnes.
G.stōnes.stōne.
D.stōne.stōnen.

Before the end of the M.E. period, however, all that survived in the sing. was the gen. -es, and in the pl. -es was used throughout for all cases. A weak gen. pl. in -ene also occurs.

The old weak declension included all three genders. Masculines have -a in nom. sing. and -an in the other cases, the pl. ran nom. and acc. -an, gen. -ena, dat. -um (like strong nouns).

The neuter weak declension was the same, except that nom. and acc. sing. ended in -e; the feminine had -e in nom. sing., otherwise was declined exactly like the masculine. In M.E. the sing. of all genders has -e in nom., -en in the other cases; the pl. -en in all cases but the gen., which ends in -ene.

Here, again, we soon find the suffix -en used simply to express plural number.

The weak gen. pl. -ene was sometimes retained for convenience, fairly late, and is often used in early texts with nouns which otherwise took the strong pl. suffix -es in the nom. pl.—alre Kingene King occurs in a twelfth-century homily (Morris, O.E. Homilies, second series, p. 89, l. 16).

Of the two types of declension, the strong predominates greatly in the North and Midlands, while the weak is far more frequent in the South, where it is extended to words which were originally strong. At the present day the Berkshire dialect uses primrosen and housen in addition to the other scattered waifs of this declension which survive in the Standard language.

Verbs.—Among the most characteristic dialectal distinctions in M.E. are the personal endings of the pres. indic. of verbs. They are as follows:

North: -e or -es in first, and -es in all other persons sing. and pl.

Midlands: first -e, second -est, third -eth; pl. -en in all persons.

Southern: first -e, second -(e)st, third -(e)th; pl. -eth in all persons.

The present participle ends in -and(e) in the North, end(e) in the Midlands, ind(e) in the South.

The suffix -ing(e), originally that whereby verbal nouns were formed (O.E. -ung, as in leornung, etc.), gradually replaces the older -ind(e) as the suffix of present participles, THE DEFINITE ARTICLE although the former continued to be used in the South down to the middle of the fourteenth century, while the old ending -and was still preserved in the North considerably later—e.g., syngand, sayand, plesand, etc., are still used by Sir David Lyndsay in a passage of some twenty verses given by Mr. Gregory Smith in Specimens of Middle Scots, pp. 162, 163, by the side of forms in -ing.

Pronouns.—The distinctions of gender and case expressed by the O.E. demonstrative pronoun, also used as a definite article, se, se̅o̅, þœt, were considerably impaired in M.E. The Northern and Midland dialects very early use the new form þe (where the þ is due to the analogy of the other cases and genders) as an indeclinable article in all cases and for all genders of the sing. the pl. is þa. In the South, however, the distinctions of gender and case are preserved much longer. A new fem. nom. sing. þe̅o̅ was formed to replace the old fem. se̅o̅ by the side of masc. þe, and þet, corresponding to O.E. þœt, was used before neuter words.

In the North þet was used as a demonstrative pronoun, indeclinable, with a pl. þās.

Traces of the original inflections still survive in a few fossilized forms, e.g., the proper name Atterbury—M.E. at þer(e) bury, O.E. œt þǣre byriġ, the change from at þer to atter being quite normal in M.E.; for the nonce = M.E. for þe nōnes = for þen ōnes, where þen is properly a dative, O.E. þǣm, levelled under the accusative, O.E. þone, ōnes being a genitive in form, used first adverbially, but here as a substantive. The neuter article survives in Sc. the tane and the tither, originally M.E. þet āne, þet ōþer. The t’other was perfectly polite colloquial English a hundred years ago, though now felt as a vulgarism when used seriously.

The Rise of Literary English.

The works written in this country down to the third quarter of the fifteenth century show more or less strongly marked points of divergence in the form of language, according to the province in which they were written. These differences are observable in the vocabulary, more strongly still in the inflexions, and most characteristically of all in the sound system, so far as this can be reconstructed from the spelling.

From the period at which Caxton’s activities begin (1475), the dialectal variety, which had hitherto been so remarkable a feature, disappears, to all intents and purposes, from literature. Henceforth the language of books becomes uniform, the spelling, owing to the necessity for comparative consistency felt by the printers, rapidly crystallizes, and the form of language thus displayed differs but little in its written form from that of the present day, of which it is, indeed, the lineal ancestor.

This literary dialect, to which Caxton by his copious industry gave wide currency and permanence, was not a bogus form of speech, deliberately vamped together from various written or spoken sources. It represents a living, spoken form of language, that of the Capital.

The London Dialect.—This dialect can be traced from the middle of the thirteenth century, in proclamations, charters, and wills—that is, both in public and private DIALECTAL CHARACTER OF LITERARY ENGLISH documents. The earliest forms are distinctly Southern in character, but Midland influence gains ground, and even Northern features find their way into the latest charters of the fifteenth century. Kentish influence is considerable, but the Saxon elements are more and more eliminated.

The language of literature and the Standard spoken English of the present day, while mainly Midland, or, rather, traceable to a M.E. Midland type, yet preserve Northern, Saxon, and Kentish elements in isolated cases. It is contended by Morsbach (Über den Ursprung der neuenglischen Schriftsprache, Heilbronn, 1888)—(1) that this composite dialect developed naturally in the Metropolis owing to social and political conditions; (2) that this is proved by an investigation of the official and legal documents in English emanating from London during the fourteenth and fifteenth centuries; (3) this dialect gradually spread its influence as a literary medium far and wide, until it became the only recognised form for writers from all provinces. Caxton, who translated several important works, such as Trevisa’s version of Higden, into the London dialect, greatly contributed to the spread of this form of speech.

Dibelius, in John Capgrave und die englische Schriftsprache, Anglia, xxiii., p. 152, etc., argues that not only in London, but in Oxford also, the tendency arose to set up a fixed literary form of English. Wycliffe, a Yorkshireman by birth, who became Master of Balliol, chose the Oxford type as his literary vehicle. The differences between the London and Oxford types persisted down to the third quarter of the fifteenth century. Both types were imitated throughout the country, and documents from Norfolk, Suffolk, and Worcester all show, by the side of local peculiarities, certain points of agreement with both the Oxford and the London forms of English. These points of agreement become stronger as time goes on, showing that the standards of both places were followed over a wide area. The knowledge of the London English, before printing, would naturally spread through the influence of the law and legislature; that of Oxford would be carried far and wide by the clergy. In this way the path was prepared for the universal acceptance of a literary form which combined the features of both the Oxford and the London models. Such a form, Dibelius maintains, is to be found in the printed works of Caxton, and such a form exists in Present-day English, which is the descendant of the dialect employed by Caxton. The great writer of the Oxford type of English was Wycliffe, whose translation of the Bible contributed to give currency to that form, and this influence may be detected among some of the writers of the Paston Letters. Dibelius, while laying stress upon the English of Oxford as an important element in the literary dialect, admits freely that the London type predominates, and that its influence is found everywhere, even in writings which show no trace of Oxford influence. Caxton’s English is far more that of London than of Oxford, and probably what of the latter element is found in his works is due to literature rather than to direct contact.

The language of Chaucer deviates in many respects from the typical London dialect of the charters, and the modern English literary language is nearer to the latter CHAUCER AND CAXTON than to the former. The explanation probably is that, although Chaucer certainly wrote in one form of the London speech of his day, the particular variety of this which he employed was the courtly language of the upper strata of society. His writings seem to represent an actual contemporary form of language rather than a literary tradition. The language actually preserved in the London wills and charters is most probably, to a certain extent, stereotyped, and the same may well be true of the Oxford type as represented by Wycliffe. Chaucer’s language contains more Southern (Saxon), and probably also more Kentish elements than that form which was to become the ancestor of Present-day English. Strong though the literary influence of Chaucer was, it was not sufficient to found a permanent type of literary language, in spite of his numerous imitators and followers. We must, indeed, suppose that a Court dialect is a more transitory type of speech, more liable to the modifying effects of fashion, than the speech of the educated middle class. It would appear that the form adopted by Caxton in his writings was so vigorous and full of vitality, as a spoken language also, that it was confirmed, consolidated, and, when necessary, subsequently rejuvenated from the spoken form. Just as the written forms of this dialect rapidly ousted and replaced the other English dialects for purposes of public and private written documents, such as wills, letters, and documents of all kinds, no less than in purely literary productions, so also, though this was a slower process, and one not yet complete, the spoken form became the standard language of the learned, the polite, and the fashionable, to the gradual elimination of provincial speech.

In addition to the authorities referred to above, the student may, with great profit, consult Ten Brink, Chaucer’s Sprache und Verskunst, Leipzig, 1899, and the remarks on pp. 20-29 of Kaluza’s Historische Grammatik der englischen Spr., vol. i., Berlin, 1900.


CHAPTER XIV

CHANGES IN ENGLISH PRONUNCIATION DURING THE MODERN PERIOD—THE DEVELOPMENT OF ENGLISH SOUNDS FROM THE FIFTEENTH CENTURY TO THE PRESENT DAY

The Problem.

It is proposed in this chapter to attempt to trace the development of the English language, more particularly of the Standard dialect, so far as the pronunciation is concerned, through the sixteenth, seventeenth, and eighteenth centuries, and to inquire by what paths of change the sounds of late M.E. passed into those forms which they now have in English speech.

During the five hundred years which have elapsed since the death of Chaucer very remarkable and far-reaching changes have taken place in the Standard language, and of these we may distinguish two main features. Firstly, the actual sounds, especially the vowels, have undergone considerable shifting; and secondly, from the materials at our disposal, it is possible to establish the fact that in most words more than one type of pronunciation of the vowels has always existed, and that that type which at one period is considered the ‘correct’ one, at a subsequent date is often discarded in favour of another type, or its descendant, which a former age would have regarded as ‘ill-bred,’ ‘vulgar,’ or ‘incorrect.’

The task of the reconstruction of the pronunciation of English during the different epochs of the Modern Period is of a different nature from that of establishing the sounds of Old and Middle English. In the latter case we have a variegated orthography which differs from dialect to dialect, in some cases from scribe to scribe, in the efforts to express the sound. The problem is to interpret the written symbols: in the former case we have a conventional spelling which is practically fixed, and such varieties as exist throw but little light upon the changes of pronunciation. On the other hand, we have in the Modern Period, for the first time, a series of systematic attempts, from various motives, to describe the actual sounds used and their distribution. The problem, therefore, is mainly how to interpret rightly the accounts given by contemporaries of the pronunciation of the various generations. It is unquestionable that in this task we obtain help from knowledge gathered indirectly by a study of the changing spelling of M.E., just as this knowledge is itself often supplemented and confirmed by the categorical statements of sixteenth or seventeenth century writers.

The Sources of our Knowledge of the Pronunciation of the Sixteenth, Seventeenth, and Eighteenth Centuries.

From the year 1530 onwards there exists a series of works by English writers in English, French, Welsh, and Latin which deal directly or incidentally with the pronunciation of English during the age in which the writers lived. These men belonged to several different classes of society; there were Divines, some of whom were Bishops FIRST STEPS IN RECONSTRUCTION and Court Chaplains, Oxford and Cambridge Professors and Heads of Houses, Schoolmasters of various ranks; there were Poets, Scholars, and Men of Science.

The late A. J. Ellis, to whom belongs the glory of having first made use of such writers as the above for our present purpose, and of having ferreted out many a long-forgotten tract, gives in Part I. of his wonderful work on Early English Pronunciation, Chapter I., an interesting account of his first struggles to interpret the accounts given by the above-mentioned phonetic authorities. His first certain guide to sixteenth-century pronunciation was derived from the works of William Salesbury, who in 1547 published a Welsh and English Dictionary, in the Introduction to which, according to Ellis, ‘about 150 typical English words’ are transcribed ‘into Welsh letters.’ The same writer also produced in 1567 a tract upon the pronunciation of Welsh, in which he refers to many other languages, thus establishing for the modern reader the pronunciation of sixteenth-century Welsh. It can thus be shown that the pronunciation of Welsh has changed very little since Salesbury’s time, and his transliterations of English words into Welsh spelling are therefore of the highest value in ascertaining the English pronunciation of his day. Salesbury’s essays are published in extenso by Ellis, together with an English translation of the Welsh treatise, in E.E.P., p. 743, etc. An even earlier phonetic transliteration of English into Welsh spelling, that of a Hymn to the Virgin, made about 1500 (cf. Sweet, H.E.S., p. 203), was published in the Transactions of the Philological Society, 1880-1881.

The following is a selection of the principal authorities, a fuller list of which is given in Ellis’s E.E.P., Part I., p. 31, etc., and Sweet’s H.E.S., p. 204, etc.:

Sixteenth-century Authorities.

1530. Palsgrave: L’esclarcissement de la langue Francoyse.

[Palsgrave was a graduate of Cambridge, and tutor to Princess Mary, sister of Henry VIII., and later on a Royal Chaplain. He died in 1554. He spoke the form of English in vogue at Court. His book contains an elaborate account of French pronunciation, elucidated by reference to English and Italian.]

1545. Meigret: Traité touchant le commun usage de l’escriture francoise.

[This book deals with French pronunciation, and makes the pronunciation of Palsgrave’s English analogues more secure.]

1547. Salesbury: A Dictionary of Englishe and Welshe.

[Salesbury was born in Denbighshire, and studied at Oxford. See reference to this book and to Ellis’s account of it above.]

1555. Cheke (Sir John): De pronunciatione Grœcœ.

[Cheke was born at Cambridge in 1514, and moved in the best literary society. He was Secretary of State in 1552, and died in 1557. In his treatise several Greek sounds are illustrated by English words spelled phonetically in Greek letters.]

1567. Salesbury: A playne and familiar Introduction teaching how to pronounce the letters in the Brytishe Tongue, now commonly called Welsh.

[All the important portions of this book reprinted by Ellis; see references above.]

1568. Smith (Sir Thomas): De recta et emendata linguæ SIXTEENTH-CENTURY AUTHORITIES anglicæ scriptione.

[Smith was born in 1515 at Saffron Walden, Essex. He was a Fellow of Queen’s College, Cambridge, public orator, and in 1536 became Provost of Eton. He was a Secretary of State in 1548, Privy Councillor in 1571. He died in 1577. The object of the above book was to improve English spelling. It contains tables of words printed in a phonetic alphabet.]

1569. Hart: An Orthographie: conteyning the due order and reason, howe to write or painte thimage of mannes voice, most like to the life or Nature. By J. H. Chester.

[Hart was the real name of the writer of this book, according to the catalogue of the British Museum. Hart was, according to Ellis, probably a Welshman. Phonetic symbols are used in the above work, and the author was acquainted with several languages. He favours a pronunciation which was in his day only coming in. Gill, writing more than fifty years later, says of Hart: ‘Sermonem nostrum characteribus suis non sequi sed ducere meditabatur.’]

1580. Bullokar: Booke at large for the Amendment of Orthographie for English Speech.

[Bullokar uses phonetic spelling. The pronunciation which he records is archaic, and agrees more with that of Palsgrave than with that of his own immediate contemporaries.]

1619 and 1621. Gill: Logonomia Anglica.

[Gill was born in Lincolnshire in 1564 (same year as Shakespeare); member of C.C.C., Cambridge; Headmaster of St. Paul’s School, 1608; died 1635. He transcribes passages from the Psalms and from Spenser in his phonetic alphabet, and discusses pronunciation at length. Gill is old-fashioned, and has a horror of modernisms. The pronunciation described is, on the whole, that of the middle of the sixteenth century. The work was reprinted in 1903 by Jiriczek in the series ‘Quellen und Forschungen,’ Strassburg.]

Butler: The English Grammar . . . whereto is annexed an Index of Words like and unlike.

[Butler was a member of Magdalen College, Oxford, and a country clergyman. He uses phonetic spelling. His pronunciation is that of the end of the sixteenth century, and he opposes the new pronunciation.]

Seventeenth-century Authorities.

Ben Jonson’s English Grammar is of interest on account of its author, but is of little value for our purpose.

1651. Willis (Thomas, of Thistlewood, Middlesex): Vestibulum Linguæ Latinæ. A Dictionarie for Children.

[Contains upwards of 4,000 words, supposed to be arranged according to rhyme, but in most cases, in reality, grouped according to spelling. There are a certain number of genuine rhymes which are useful.]

1653-1699. Wallis: Grammatica Linguæ Anglicanæ Cui prœfigitur De Loquela; sive de sonorum omnium loquelarium formatione: Tractatus Grammatico-Physicus.

[This book went through six editions between the above dates. Wallis was born at Ashford, in Kent, in 1616; appointed Savilian Professor of Geometry at Oxford in 1649; died, 1703. The SEVENTEENTH-CENTURY AUTHORITIES introduction is of great importance, and establishes, with considerable certainty, the value of all the symbols. This work is the chief authority for the middle of the seventeenth century.]

1668. Wilkins: An Essay towards a Real Character, and a Philosophical Language.

[Wilkins was born in Northamptonshire in 1614; graduated at Oxford in 1648; elected Warden of Wadham, 1648; Bishop of Ripon, 1668; died, 1672. This ‘Essay’ contains an admirable treatise on Phonetics. Wilkins makes use of a phonetic alphabet, into which he transliterates the Lord’s Prayer and the Creed. The book is not infrequently to be met with in booksellers’ catalogues of the present day.]

1668. Price: English Orthographie is the beginning of a very long title, which includes, among other things, ‘Also some Rules for the points and pronunciation.’

[The book, when used by the side of other authorities, is useful ‘in discriminating the exact sounds of the different vowel digraphs of the seventeenth century.’]

1685. Cooper: Grammatica Linguæ Anglicanæ.

[This book contains a treatise on speech sounds, a discussion of peculiarities of orthography and pronunciation, and long lists of words illustrating the several vowel sounds.]

1688. Miege: The Great French Dictionary.

[Valuable information as to pronunciation prefixed to each letter.]

Eighteenth-Century Authorities.

1701. Jones (John): Practical Phonography. (The first words of an immense title.)

[A kind of pronouncing dictionary, in which all kinds of pronunciations of the same words are given, and therefore valuable as recording what actually occurred in English speech at the beginning of the eighteenth century.]

Circa 1713. Anonymous: Grammar of the English Tongue.

[Useful in corroboration of the statements of other authorities of the period.]

1725. Lediard: Grammatica Anglicana Critica, in which English words are transliterated phonetically into German spelling. Ellis gives a full account of results (Part IV., p. 1040, etc.).

1766. Buchanan: Essay towards establishing a standard for an elegant and uniform pronunciation of the English Language throughout the British Dominions.

[The work of a Scotsman, this book bears some traces of this in the pronunciation described. Ellis notes that on the whole, however, this does not differ materially from that heard in the middle of the nineteenth century, except inasmuch as certain pronunciations of certain words are given as ‘learned and polite’ which would not now be so accounted.]

A tract by Dr. Benjamin Franklin, entitled A Scheme for a New Alphabet and Reformed Mode of Spelling, in the form of a correspondence between himself and a lady, is given by Ellis (pp. 1058, etc.). The correspondence was carried on in the proposed alphabet, and the tract contains a table of sounds and symbols, and remarks by Franklin thereupon. Ellis prints the paper in full, but unfortunately turns the whole thing into his own very clumsy Palœotype.

Method of using the Authorities.—By comparing the THE INTERPRETATION OF THE AUTHORITIES statements of a considerable number of contemporary authorities with regard to the pronunciation of a given sound, weighing one against another, and checking and interpreting one by another, we attempt first to arrive at a conclusion as to what is the precise sound which the various writers are trying to describe. The result of such an investigation often leads to the conclusion that at the same period there was more than one pronunciation of the same word; the writers are manifestly describing different sounds, though dealing with the same symbol. We thus establish the existence of two or more types of pronunciation at the same period. These varieties may arise from several causes. They may be the descendants of doublets which arose at an earlier period; they may represent different dialectal treatments of the same original sound; they may represent the pronunciation of the older and younger generation respectively. When the existence of the several types at a given period is once definitely established, the next problem is to inquire which earlier type each represents, and into which later form it subsequently develops. Until we have done this we can form no true idea of the development of any particular sound. Hence it is of the highest importance to know all the pronunciations of a given word which existed at a given time. If we find that ‘blood’ was pronounced (blūd) In the sixteenth and seventeenth centuries, we are not justified in concluding, without further evidence, that the modern form (blad) is its lineal descendant. This would be tantamount to asserting that seventeenth-century (ū) appears as (a) in the nineteenth, a statement which would at once be disproved by further examination. The problem resolves itself into showing (1) what sixteenth-century sound was the ancestor of Present-day (a), and (2) what is the Present-day representative of the sixteenth and seventeenth century (ū). When we find that a very large number of words which now contain the sound (a) were pronounced with (ŭ) in the sixteenth century, and with that sound alone, we should be inclined to say that the former sound has been developed from the latter, and further to postulate a sixteenth-century pronunciation (blŭd) as the ancestor of the Present-day polite form of the word. As a matter of fact, the pronunciation (blŭd) can be shown to have existed in the sixteenth century by the side of (blūd). Similarly, although we can show that in the eighteenth century, in good society, people said (Kwæliti) and (Kwæntiti), it would be quite erroneous to suppose that these particular forms developed into the Present-day (Kwoliti) and (Kwontiti). The former types have simply been discarded, and their places have been taken by others whose predecessors existed in the eighteenth century side by side with those first mentioned, although at that time they did not happen to be the forms in fashionable use.

In a word, when tracing the history of a language we must always bear in mind the twofold problem: first, the development of the actual sounds themselves, and, secondly, the changing fashion of using them in a given dialect in a particular group of words.

Ellis and Sweet both give the statements of the various MODERN DEVELOPMENT OF M.E. Ā̆ authorities, so that the student can draw his own conclusions, in which he will, however, receive great help from the discussion of every point by the above-mentioned scholars. Ellis, besides the words in the text, has copious pronouncing vocabularies of the sixteenth, seventeenth, and eighteenth centuries, compiled from the whole body of Orthographists, Phoneticians, and Dictionary-makers of those centuries. In these lists all the variants in each period are given, and they are of the greatest use as affording convenient material for phonological investigation.

The Sounds in Detail.

In the present case the most convenient way of dealing with the subject will be to start with the M.E. sound and trace it downwards to the present day.

By way of illustration of the kind of material upon which our conclusions are based, and also of the method of dealing with it, it will be as well to give the full statements of the contemporary authorities concerning M.E. a and ā. The development of the remaining sounds will be given without reference to these, but each statement is based upon the same kind of material as that given in the case of a and ā.

The rules of pronunciation as given by the authorities are always based upon the uses of the letters.

Palsgrave (1530): ‘The soundyng of a which is most generally used throughout the frenche tonge is such as we use with vs, where the best englysche is spoken, whichϵ is lyke as the Italians sound a, or as they with vs, that pronounce the latine tonge aryght.’

This points to a mid-back-slack for ‘the best English.’ Possibly the other sound of a which Palsgrave implies also existed in his day was a fronted form—almost our (æ).

Salesbury (1547): ‘A in English is of the same sound as a in Welsh, as is evident in these words of English—all, aal, pale, paal, sale, sal.’

The double vowels here imply length, and the last word should have been transcribed saal. The sound of a in Welsh at present is (a) mid-back-slack, whether long or short. He invariably transcribes M.E. ā with aa, and M.E. a with ae, apart from occasional inconsistencies like the above: babe he writes baab, bake, baak, plague, plaag, etc. Examples of short a are papp, nag, fflacs (flax), etc.

Smith (1568) says the only sounds of English a are those of long and short Latin a.

As samples of short a he has: man, far, hat, mar, pass, examples of the long are: mane, farewell, hate, mare, pace, bare, bake. Since Salesbury gives the last word with (ā), there can be little doubt what sound Smith implied by ‘sonus a vocalis Romanæ longæ.’ The first group had the same sound short.

Hart (1569) identifies English a with that of German, Italian, French, Spanish, and Welsh, which is to be pronounced ‘with wyde opening the mouth, as when a man yauneth.’

Butler (1633): ‘A is in English, as in all other languages, the first vowel, and the first letter of the Alphabet; the which . . . hath two sounds, one when it is short, another when long, as in man and mane, hat and hate.’

This is the first indication of a distinction in quality between long and short a, and it is not repeated till fifty years later, by Cooper. It seems clear that Butler must FRONTING OF M.E. Ā have heard a difference, however, and since both long and short are certainly fronted a little later, it seems probable that one may have been slightly in advance of the other in reaching (æ). Again, since M.E. long ā has not only been fronted, but also raised to (ϵ̄, ē, ɛi) in later English, we shall perhaps be justified in assuming that Butler pronounced (hat) hat, but (hǣt) hate. If so, he must have been rather in advance of other contemporary writers, and must have described the pronunciation just coming in. Palsgrave’s implied statement of the existence of another sound of a, than of full-mid-back sound, may have referred to this fronted form, which in his day was apparently not highly esteemed, and may have originated in provincial speech.

The net result of the above statements seems to be that M.E. a, long or short, was retained throughout the sixteenth and well into the seventeenth century. The fronting tendency began in the sixteenth century, but was considered first as a vulgarism, and then as new-fangled, until the first quarter of the seventeenth century.

Middle English ‘a’ in Seventeenth-Century Pronunciation.

Ben Jonson (1640): ‘A with us in most words is pronounced lesse than the French à, as in art, act, apple, ancient. But when it comes before l in the end of a syllable, it obtaineth the full French sound, and is uttered with the mouth and throat wide open’d, the tongue bent back from the teeth, as in al, smal, gal, fall, tal, cal.’

The first of these statements, that a ‘is lesse than the French à,’ seems to indicate that Ben Jonson followed the (then) new fashion, and pronounced a fronted (a), though perhaps not yet (æ). The a before l was clearly a full-back vowel, whether mid or low it is impossible to say. The pronunciation of all, small, gall, etc., here described is not that which produced Present-day Standard English (ɔ̄l, smɔ̄l), etc. We shall deal with that under the M.E. au.

Wallis (1653-1699) represents fully-developed, typical seventeenth-century pronunciation. He describes English a as ‘a exile,’ and goes on: ‘Quale auditur in vocibus, bat, vespertilio; bate, discordia; pal, palla episcopalis; pale, pallidus; Sam (Samuelis contractio); same, idem; lamb, agnus; lame, claudus; dam, mater (brutosum); dame, domina; bar, vectis; bare, nudus; ban, exsecror; bane, pernicies, etc. Differt hic sonus a Germanorum â pingui seu aperto; eo quod Angli linguæ medium elevent, adeoque aërem in Palato comprimant; Germani vero linguæ medium deprimant, adeoque aërem comprimant in gutture. Galli fere sonum illum proferunt ubi e præcedit literam m vel n, in eadem syllaba ut entendement,’ etc.

This vowel (a) has previously been classified by Wallis as one of those of which he says: ‘Vocales Patinæ in Palato formantur, aëre scilicet inter palati et linguæ medium moderate compresso’; and distinguishing the particular vowel he says: ‘Majori apertura formatur Anglorum a, hoc est á exile.’

This description must refer to the same sound as that which Ben Jonson says is ‘lesse than the French à,’ and is pretty clearly fixed by Wallis as the low-front, being made by the ‘middle of the tongue’ and with ‘a greater opening’ than the other front vowels. It will be noticed that the English words in the passage quoted above are alternately short and long, and must therefore be (æ), as in RISE OF THE MODERN Æ SOUND (bæt), and (ǣ), as in (bǣt), respectively.

Wilkins (1668) says of a ‘that it is framed by an emission of the breath, betwixt the tongue and the concave of the palate; the upper superfices of the tongue being rendered less concave, and at a less distance from the palate.’

Wilkins’ pairs of words to illustrate the short and long form of this sound are—

Short: batval-leyfatmatpalRad-nor
Long: batevalefatematepaletrade

These examples and the remarks of Wilkins which have been quoted point to the same results as in the case of Wallis.

Cooper (1685): Cooper’s account of the pronunciation of a must indeed have been considered ‘new-fangled’ by the older generation of his contemporaries. He distinguishes two sounds for original long ā, using the phrase ‘a exilis’ to designate a different sound from that referred to by previous writers when they use the expression. The following are his remarks: ‘A formatur a medio linguæ ad concavum palati paululum elevato. In his can possum, pass by prætereo, a corripitur; in cast jacio, past pro passed præteritus, producitur. Frequentissimus auditur hic sonus apud Anglos, qui semper hoc modo pronunciant a Latinum; ut in amabam. . . . Hunc sonum correptum production semper scribimus per a; at huic characteri præterea adhibentur sonus unus et alter: prior, qui pro vocali ejus longa habetur ut in cane . . . posterior ut in was sect. septima sub o gutturalem.’

This seems to imply that can and pass had (æ), cast, past (ǣ). Further, the symbol a also expresses a sound which is generally held to be the ordinary long sound (ǣ), but which is not the same; this other sound occurs in cane. Incidentally we may notice that Cooper pronounced was, not (wæz), but (woz). What was the third sound expressed by a?

Writing of ϵ, he says: ‘ϵ formatur à linguâ magis elevata et expansa, quam in a proprius ad extremitatem, unde concavum palati minus redditur sonus magis acutus; ut in ken video. . . . Vera majusce soni productio scribitur per a atque a longum falso denominatur; ut in cane, canna; wane, deflecto; and ante ge ut age, ætas; in cæteris autem vocabulis (ni fallor) omnibus ubi e quiescens ad finem syllabæ post a, adjicitur; u gutturalis . . . inseritur post a ut in name, nomen, quasi scriberetur na-um dissyllabum.’

Here we have the statement that the sound in cane, wane was the long of that in ken, and that in the two former words it was falsely called ‘long a.’ This clearly implies that the third vowel sound expressed by the symbol a was a mid-front, presumably, since it is the long of that in ken, a slack vowel = (ϵ̄). A further statement is that when this long sound stood before certain consonants a vowel glide ‘u gutturalis,’ was developed after it. Writers of this period nearly always mean by short u an unrounded vowel, probably very similar to that in Present-day but, and this sound, whatever it may have been when stressed (probably high-back-tense), may have actually existed in Cooper’s day as a glide vowel, or, as is, perhaps, more probable, the sound actually intended here is the mix-mixed-slack (ə). This THE THREE ‘A’-SOUNDS implies a pronunciation (kϵ̄ən) (nϵ̄əm), etc.

Cooper’s lists illustrating the different sounds of a are as follows:

a brevis (= æ).a longa (= ǣ).a exilis (= ɛ̄).
bar, vectis.barge, navicula.bare, nudus.
blab, effutio.blast, flatus.blazon, divulgo.
cap, pileum.carking, anxietas.cape, capa.
car, carrus.carp, carpo.care, cura.
cat, catus.cast, jactus.case, theca.
dash, allido.dart, jaculum.date, dactylus.
flash, fulguro.flasket, corbus gluus.flake, flocculus.
gash, cæsura.gasp, oscito.gate, janua.
grand, grandis.grant, concedo.grange, villa.
land, terra.lanch, solvo.lane, viculus.
mash, farrago.mask, larva.mason, lapidarius
pat, aptus.path, semita.pate, caput.
tar, pix fluida.tart, scriblitatares, lolia.

Among words which have the diphthong (ɛ̄ə), Cooper includes many which in M.E. had a diphthong ai, which was evidently levelled, in his speech under M.E. ā. The ɛ̄ə list is:

bain, balneum.hail, grando.maid, virgo.
bane, venenuum.hale, traho.made, factus.
main, magnus.lay’n, jacui.pain, dolor.
mane, juba.lane, viculus.pane, quadra.
plain, manifestas.spaid, castratus.tail, cauda.
plane, lavigo.spade, ligo.tale, fabula.

Miege (1688) confirms Cooper’s account of ē in certain words:

‘Dans la langue Anglaise cette voyelle A s’appelle et se prononce ai. Lorsqu’elle est jointe avec d’autres lettres, elle retient ce même son dans la plupart des Mots; mais il se prononce tantôt long, tantôt bref. L’a se prononce en ai long generalement lorsqu’il est suivi immediatement d’une consonne, et d’une e final. Exemple: fare, tare, care, grace, fable, qui se prononcent ainsi faire, taire, caire, graice, faible.’

Miege notes that ‘regard se prononce regaird. . . . Dans le mot de Jane l’a se prononce en e masculin, Dgéne.’

The eighteenth-century authorities are very unsatisfactory in their statements regarding the fate of the three seventeenth-century sounds (æ, ǣ, ɛ̄). Apparently they were all preserved, (ɛ̄) becoming tense late in the century, and ǣ tending to be retracted towards ā, which sound it has to-day in Standard English. In Sheridan’s Dictionary, however (1780), we still find only (pǣþ), etc., and no (ā) sounds. In the course of the nineteenth century (ē) was diphthongized in Standard English to (ϵi), in which the first element is half tense. In the Cockney dialect of London, and often in Liverpool and Manchester, this has become (æi) or (ai), according to the social class of the speaker.

We may now summarize the results of the foregoing inquiry. M.E. a and ā were preserved on the whole throughout the sixteenth century, although the fronting process may have begun here and there before the end of the century. In the seventeenth century the fronting process was completed, (a) becoming (æ), as at present, (ā) becoming (ǣ). In the course of the century (ǣ) was raised to (ɛ̄). Before certain combinations (æ) was lengthened during this century. This lengthening does not affect all words of the same class, therefore we must suppose that in some cases forms from other dialects were adopted by speakers of the Standard language. It seems to take place chiefly before s and r followed by another consonant, HISTORY OF M.E. Ā̆ SUMMARIZED and before (þ and ð)—e.g., (kǣrt, gǣsp, pǣþ).

This new long (ǣ) was not levelled under the old long (from M.E. ā), since this had already become (ɛ̄). Concrete examples of the development of M.E. ā̆ are:

{bat}      
M.E. a{rer}      
{baþ       
         
=17th}(bæt).     
cent. (æ)}(ræðer) (rǣðer)}18th19th}(rāðər).
(bæþ) < (bǣþ)}cent. (ǣ);cent. (ā)}(bāþ).

 

{fāce}          
M.E. ā{nāme}          
{ðer}          
             
(fǣs)}{ (fϵ̄s)}18th(fēs)}19th(feis).
=17th(nǣm)}<{ (nϵ̄m)}<cent.(nēm)}cent.(neim).
cent.(rǣðer)}{ (rϵ̄ðer)}ɛ < ē(rēðer)}ei(reiðə).

The origin of the M.E. doublets raðer, ðer, faðer, ðer, have already been explained in the chapter on M.E. sound-changes (ante, p. 271). Present-day (ā) is never derived from M.E. ā, which is always (ɛi), but from M.E. a with seventeenth-century lengthening.

The seventeenth and eighteenth century sound (ǣ) is still preserved in many of the Southern English dialects, and in the Irish brogue, where such pronunciations as (kǣrd), (bǣþ) are usual. In the Northern dialects the fronting of M.E. a was never fully carried out, and (a) is either preserved as a full-back or is only slightly advanced. The seventeenth-century lengthening does not seem to have affected these dialects, which have the same vowel in (man, baþ, ka(r)d), etc.

The Present-day forms ‘clerk’ (klāk); ‘Derby’ (dābi); (hāþ) hearth; (hāt), heart, may be discussed here. Originally, both of these words had M.E. erclerk, Derbi. But in M.E. e before r was often made into a, doubtless through an intermediate stage (æ). This has happened in star, far, where the old spelling has been retained. In these words we have the sixteenth-century (a), seventeenth-century (æ), then (ǣ}), which, as we have seen, becomes (ā) in Late English. Our pronunciation of clerk and Derby, heart, hearth, etc., goes back, in each case, to a M.E. (a), which has regularly become (ā) in Late English by the stages mentioned. The spelling in these words is that of another M.E. type, with (ɛ) or (ɛ̄), which before r becomes (ʌ̄) quite regularly in Late English. The provincial or ‘vulgar’ (dʌ̄bi, klʌ̄k, hʌ̄þ) go back to the M.E. (ɛr) type. In other words, Standard English preserves this type; thus (sʌ̄vent), servant; (hʌ̄d), heard; (lʌ̄n), learn, are derived from M.E. pronunciations with (ɛr, ɛ̄r). In eighteenth-century colloquial literature these words are sometimes spelled larn, sarvant, which expresses a then common pronunciation (lǣrn, sǣrvent), etc., and these forms are established by seventeenth and eighteenth century authorities. In polite speech, however, only the (ʌ̄) forms survive in these words. The spelling Clark in the proper name, of course, implies the same type as that which is now received as ‘correct.’ It is one of those sports of fashion so common in the history of a Class Dialect that (klʌ̄k, dʌbi) should now be considered vulgar, and (sāvənt) equally so.

M.E. (ϵ) and (ϵ̄) and (ē).—The short, slack M.E. (ϵ) has survived in English pronunciation to the present day. It occurs in such words as men, better, set, etc., and in friend (frend), where it is the result of a M.E. shortening of ē, which subsequently lost its tenseness, probably also in breath, from M.E. (brɛþ) from (brɛ̄þ), from earlier brǣþ. The unshortened form is heard in ‘breathe,’ M.E. brɛ̄ðen.

The symbol e in M.E. also denoted two distinct long vowels, as we have seen (above, p. 259, etc.).

1. (ϵ̄), which had two origins: (a) O.E. œ̄, M.E. hēþ, from HISTORY OF M.E. TENSE Ē O.E. hœ̄þ; (b) O.E. e, lengthened during M.E. period in open syllables: bēren ‘bear,’ O.E. beran; mēte, ‘meat,’ O.E. mete.

2. (ē), which sprang from—(a), O.E. ē, whatever its origin, as in hēr, ‘here’; , ‘he’; sēd (now W. Sax.), ‘seed’; quēn, O.E. cwēn; (b) O.E. ēo, as in , ‘bee,’ O.E. bēo; frē, ‘free’; O.E. frēo. (c) Kentish e (from y), lengthened in M.E. open syllables, as in ēvel, ‘evil,’ O. Kt. efel, W.S., etc., yfel. (d) O.E. ē, from original e lengthened before -ld, etc., during the O.E. period, as in M.E. schēld, ‘shield,’ O.E. scēld, earlier sċĕld; M.E. fēld ‘field’; O.E. fēld, earlier fĕld. (e) Anglo-French ē as in chēfe, chiefe, appēren, appiēren.

We may conveniently deal first with the development of M.E. tense ē. The earliest sixteenth-century authorities show that before the middle of the century this sound had already been raised to the high-front-tense (ī). The words which appear in the pages of these writers as having unmistakably (ī) are: he, we, me, she, bee, bier, peer, cheese, chief, field, ease, lief, sheep, trees, queen, friend, feet, sheet, meet, geese, deed, weary, greet, ween, green, to wet (Levins’ Manipulus).

These all agree with the Present-day Standard English, except friend—at present (frɛnd), which is from a M.E. shortened form—though Scotch has (frīnd)—and to wet. Our (wɛt) is a M.E. shortening of the O.E. wœ̄tan, M.E. (wɛ̄ten), and apparently preserves the Saxon form, whereas sixteenth-century (wīt), like Mod. Sc. ‘weet,’ goes back to an Old Anglian wētan, which preserved its tense vowel in M.E. and underwent no shortening—at any rate not until quite recently. Whenever we find evidence of this raising to (ī) in sixteenth century, we must assume a form with tense (ē) in M.E. Most words of this class were spelled already in the sixteenth century with ce, in distinction to those with M.E. (ɛ̄), written ea. The sound thus developed undergoes no further change beyond the fact that in words like ‘bier’ a vowel glide has developed after the (ī) before the r, which was subsequently lost in pronunciation, while (ī) has become (i) in Standard English: (biə), etc.

This raising of (ē) to (ī) could not have taken place until the old ī of O. and M.E. had been diphthongized, otherwise the new (ī) would have shared its fate.

The Treatment of M.E. Slack (ϵ̄).—After the raising of (ē), (ϵ̄) was gradually made tense, and thus a new (ē) arose. The raising of this sixteenth-century (e) to (ī) did not, apparently, take place in the received pronunciation before the eighteenth century, but it must have occurred among some speakers as early as the first quarter of the seventeenth century, since Gill complains of a foppish pronunciation of meat as (mīt) instead of (mēt), and (līv), leave, instead of (lēv). This is not merely a case of an old-fashioned speaker objecting to a new pronunciation which was already well established, since the change did not become widespread till much later. It is impossible to say whether this seventeenth-century raising of the new (ē) had its origin in a provincial or a class dialect, but in any case it is a good example of the fact that what is deemed, at one period, an affected pronunciation often represents a genuine tendency of language, which later on becomes universal.

It is interesting to note that the Irish brogue retains the seventeenth and eighteenth century pronunciations of M.E. (ϵ̄), as (ē); (hēt), heat, (sē), sea, (trēt), treat, (bēt) beat, (konsēl), conceal, (dēl), deal, etc., are all regular seventeenth THE (AI) AND (OI) SOUNDS and eighteenth century pronunciations, which are still heard in Ireland.

Standard English retains (ē) as (ϵi) in a few words: great, break—where, perhaps, the r may have prevented raising—and steak, which must, perhaps, be regarded as a provincial survival. Curiously enough, (brīk) is quite a common pronunciation in Ireland to-day, and this form and (grīt) are both recorded for the eighteenth century. The vowel in head, dead, bread, red, etc., which in M.E. was (ɛ̄), is the result of an Early Modern shortening. The unshortened forms are heard in Sc. (hīd, dīd), etc., where the normal eighteenth-century raising has taken place. The shortening of the vowel in these words which is common in Sc. must be quite recent.

M.E. ī and oi.—The former sound has invariably become the diphthong (ai) in Present-day English. That the process must have begun in the first quarter of the sixteenth century is certain, as we have already indicated, from the fact that Palsgrave (1530) distinctly identifies the pronunciation of M.E. (ē) with that of French ī, which latter, he says, is pronounced ‘almost as we sound e with vs.’ It is curious that, although Palsgrave implies a difference between French and English ī, he does not definitely suggest that the latter is a diphthong, and neither Smith, Bullokar, nor Gill hint at all clearly at diphthongal pronunciation. On the other hand, in the Hymn to the Virgin ī is transliterated ei in ei = Iabeiding, abiding, Kreist, Christ; and Salesbury writes vein for vine, ddein, thine, deitses (daitʃɛz) for the provincial pronunciation of ‘ditch,’ etc. Hart also writes eireid bei, ‘ride by,’ which leaves no doubt that these writers recognised the diphthongal character of the sound. In the next century the first element is identified by Wilkins as the sound in but, which, as we shall see, had in his day already a pronunciation not far removed from the present sound, probably that of rather a higher back vowel. Holder states that the sound is a diphthong composed of a, i, or e, i. Cooper gives the same account of the sound as Wilkins, and Miege says the best way of describing the sound is by the two vowels a and i.

An important point is that both Cooper and Jones identify the sound of ī in wine, guide, with that of oi in joint, broil, etc. In this connection we may note that Pope rhymes join with line. (Cf. p. 67 above.)

The meaning of all this is that M.E. ī from early in the sixteenth century underwent a process of diphthongization, and by the last half of the seventeenth century had reached the stage (ai) or (əi), in which stage it was identical with the contemporary pronunciation of the old French diphthong oi (in joy, join, etc.). This accounts for Pope’s rhyme above. Henceforth the normal development of both classes of words would, of course, have been the same, and Present-day English shows the last stage in that development in the diphthong (ai) in (waif, lain, fain, taim), etc., wife, line, fine, time, etc. In the other class of words, however, those with old oi, the old diphthong has been artificially reintroduced through the influence of the spelling; hence line and join no longer rhyme in Standard English. In Vulgar and Dialectal English, however, the old oi has pursued its normal course of development, and has become (ai), just as old ī has. Hence we get the ‘vulgar’ (bail, džain, ail), etc., which comic writers express by the spellings bile, jine, ile, for boil, join, oil, etc. Here THE TWO ‘Ō’ SOUNDS again the Irish brogue preserves the eighteenth-century sound, and has (ai) or (əi) in both classes of words, which is the explanation of the popular belief, in this country, that an Irishman calls himself what the humorous writers spell as ‘Oirishman,’ and that he pronounces (woif, foiv, ʃoin) for wife, five, shine, etc. The eighteenth-century pronunciation of this diphthong is approximately preserved also in Oxfordshire and Berkshire.

M.E. ō.—The symbol o represented two distinct long vowels in M.E.: (a) The old tense ō, as in gōd, ‘good’; blōd, ‘blood’; sōna, ‘soon,’ etc.; (b) a slack vowel with an o-quality, and which had two origins: (1) the rounding of O.E. ā, as in stōn, ‘stone,’ O.E. stān; old, O.E. āld; and (2) the lengthening of O.E. ŏ in open syllables, as in þrōte, ‘throat,’ O.E. þrŏtu; ōpen, O.E. ŏpen, etc. The slack sound was often written oa in M.E., but not with perfect regularity, and the tense was frequently written oo to express length, but this symbol is very often written for the long slack also, as in stoon, etc.

Development of M.E. tense ō.—This sound, originally probably the mid-back-tense-round, as in Modern French beau, was gradually over-rounded, passing through the stage of the Modern Swedish ō in sol, ‘sun,’ which, to unaccustomed ears, has almost the acoustic effect of (ū), and then raised until it became a fully-formed (ū).

The sixteenth-century writers on the subject leave no doubt that this stage was reached by the middle of that century. It is frankly described by the best authorities as an (ū)-sound. This sound, when once developed, either (1) remains until the present time, as in spoon, root, fool, shoe, loose, etc. (= spūn, rūt, fūl, ʃū, lūs); or (2) it has undergone (in Standard English) a recent (early nineteenth-century [?]) shortening, in which case it also becomes slack, as in good, book, wood, foot, etc. (gud, buk, wud, fut); or (3) it underwent shortening to (ŭ) already in the sixteenth century. The fate of this sixteenth-century shortening we shall discuss under the treatment of sixteenth-century and M.E. (ŭ).

[Note.Smith (1568) says that the Scots pronounce (ȳ) in cook, good, blood, hood, food, book, took, evidently referring to the same sound as is still heard in Sc. as the representative of O.E. tense ō.]

M.E. slack ō.—This sound, probably the mid-back-slack-round, was preserved in early Mod. Eng. This is confirmed by the identification of it with Welsh ō, with the Italian ‘open’ o, and as the long sound of short English o. Smith (1568) gives the pairs smocksmoke, hophope, sopsoap, notnote, robrobe, etc., as showing the short and long of the same vowel. Florio (1611) identifies the sound of Italian ‘open’ o with that in English bone, dog, God, rod, stone, tone, etc.

Gill (1621) recognises only one o-sound—short, as in coll, long, as in coal. Up to this point, after the raising and over-rounding of the old tense ō to (ū), no tense ō existed in English, only (ō). In 1653, however, Wallis recognises two long o-sounds, one identical with French au (ō), the other long a variety of that in folly, cost, etc. The former of these sounds is, of course, the tense ō, and has developed out of the long slack of the former generation. It is mentioned by Wallis as occurring in one, none, whole, coal, boat; and Wilkins also mentions an ō, obviously the same sound, which has no corresponding short sound in English, which THE RISE OF THE ‘OBSCURE’ SOUND OF M.E. Ū is found in boat, foale, vote, mote, pole, rode. Wallis’s one, none (ōn, nōn) belong, of course, to a different type of pronunciation from that used to-day in these words. Wallis’s other long o-sound is a new slack o, developed from an earlier (au), which will be discussed later.

The new middle seventeenth-century long tense o just described, derived from the earlier long slack, was preserved in English until it was diphthongized to its present various diphthongal forms in the nineteenth century.

As regards M.E. ŏ little need be said, as it has changed but little, beyond being lowered, perhaps, during the eighteenth century, from a mid to a low-back-slack-round.

M.E. ŭ.—This was, in all probability, a tense vowel, and remained unchanged down to the end of the sixteenth century. During the sixteenth century the number of words containing this sound was increased by the addition of several with a shortened form of the new (ū) from M.E. tense (ō). Among words with original ŭ which are mentioned by the sixteenth-century writers as still retaining this sound are buck, gut, lust, suffer, thunder, all of which are transliterated with w by Salesbury (bwck, gwt, etc.); but, luck, mud, full, pull, etc., and among those with the new (ū) from (ō) for which a shortened pronunciation is established are: good, flood, look, blood, book.

During the seventeenth century short u was gradually unrounded in all those words in which it occurred. This is made clear by the statements of the authorities, some of whom are at a loss to describe the new sound. Wallis says short u has an ‘obscure sound’ which resembles that of the final syllable of French serviteur; Wilkins describes it as ‘a simple letter, a pert, sonorous guttural, being framed by a free emission of breath from the throat.’ Holder gives a very definite account of what we should now call a high-back-unrounded vowel, saying that that u is an (u) sound ‘in which the lip does not concur, as in cut, full’ (kat, fal). This can only mean unrounded (u). This is the ancestor of our present sound, which has, however, been lowered from a high to a mid-back. It should be noted that in Present-day Standard the old (u) is still kept, as a rule, after lip consonants (put, pull, bull, full, etc.), though now pronounced slack, having probably been restored, if, indeed, it actually ever was unrounded, before the tongue position was lowered. This is not universally the case, however, as is seen from but, mud, punt, etc., which have the unrounded sound.

The seventeenth-century authorities are not always in agreement with Present-day polite usage as regards the distribution of the unrounded vowel, especially in words where it represents the shortened sixteenth-century (ū) from tense (ō). The following pronunciations are all recorded in the seventeenth century: from (bazəm), ‘bosom,’ (fat), ‘foot,’ (gad), ‘good,’ (had), ‘hood,’ (sat), ‘soot,’ (stad), stood, (tak), ‘took,’ (wad), ‘wood,’ (wal), ‘wool,’ all of which would be regarded as vulgar provincialisms by educated society to-day. They may, of course, still be heard in the dialects. The Standard pronunciation of to-day, in the above words, namely (fut), etc., is, of course, a later shortening, as already pointed out, of a seventeenth-century type with (ū) or perhaps with (u), since the shortened types are also recorded in late seventeenth century, and side by side with (fat), which, by the way, is THREEFOLD PRONUNCIATION OF OLD TENSE Ō designated barbare by Cooper, we get also (fut) and (fūt).

On the other hand, (ŭ) is recorded by Cooper in blood, flood, brother, where we now have (a). In any case, it would appear that fashion has decided which type of an old (M.E.) tense ō-word shall be considered as correct at the present day. Thus in ‘spoon’ (spūn) we have sixteenth-century (ū) preserved; in ‘book’ (buk) we have a seventeenth or eighteenth century shortening of this (ū); and in blood (blad), (maðə), ‘mother,’ (braðə), ‘brother,’ we have representatives of a sixteenth-century shortening of the new (ū), which, as we have seen, underwent unrounding in the following century.

There is no reason, except fashion, why (blad) should be polite, but (fat) vulgar, nor why, on the other hand, (blud) or (blud) should have vanished from educated speech.

The seventeenth-century unrounding was not carried out equally in all dialects. Thus, in Lancashire sixteenth-century ŭ was partially unrounded and lowered, and the characteristic tense sound which results is used in all cases to represent M.E. and sixteenth-century ŭ—that is, equally in cut, pull, foot, the full unrounded vowel of the Standard dialect being unknown, and also the fully rounded high-back-slack. Those sixteenth-century (ū)s which were not shortened during that century remain unchanged, as in (kūk, būk), etc.

In other forms of English, again, such as some of the Yorkshire dialects, sixteenth-century (ŭ) undergoes no unrounding at all, but remains everywhere as (ŭ), with loss of tenseness—e.g., full, cut, nut, etc. (cf. Wright, Windhill Dialect, § 111).

In Scotch dialects sixteenth-century (ŭ) has been unrounded, and has become the mid-back-tense, as in Standard English. In the Standard English as spoken in Scotland the slack sound of short (u) is unknown, and the archaic short tense sound is preserved, full and fool both having the same sound, namely high-back-tense-round, short.

In the genuine Sc. vernacular O.E. tense ō underwent a totally different development already in the M.E. period from that which it followed in Southern English.

M.E. ū.—Just before M.E. tense ō was raised to (ū), the original ū underwent the beginnings of a process of diphthongization. From Palsgrave’s remarks it would appear that already in his day there was a very slight degree of diphthongization, sufficient to distinguish the sound from the newly-developed (ū), but not enough to confuse it with the older (au) in (graunt), ‘grant,’ (faul), ‘fall’ (see below, pp. 333-336). The process of diphthongization probably consisted of, first, a sudden decrease of stress during the utterance of (ū), thus giving (ū́u) or (ūu); then the dissimilation of the two elements, possibly by partially unrounding and lowering the first element to (ō), giving (ṓu); then the complete unrounding of the first element to (ā́u); then shortening and slacking to (au), which is approximately the present pronunciation in the Standard dialect. Various vulgarisms and provincial forms of this diphthong exist, such as (æu, ϵu). In some dialects monophthonging, apparently from the (au) stage, has taken place—e.g., Windhill Dialect hās, etc., from (haus). On the other hand, the Dialect of Addlington (Lancs) has (brɛ̄n, hɛ̄s, ɛ̄, ɛ̄nd), etc., = ‘brown,’ ‘house,’ ‘how,’ ‘hound,’ where the monophthongization has apparently taken M.E. (Ȳ) BECOMES (JU) place from the (ϵu) stage. (Cf. Hargreaves, Addlington Dialect, § 12.) There is no reason to suppose that (ɛu, æu) are intermediate stages on the way to (au); they are, rather, special further developments of that sound.

M.E. ȳ written u.—The sound ȳ—that is, the high-front-tense-round—survived throughout the M.E. period. Its origins are: (1) O.E. ȳ (in the Southern or Saxon dialects); (2) Anglo-French ȳ (written u). There seems no doubt that the (ȳ) sound remained in English pronunciation down to the middle of the seventeenth century, since writers as late as Wallis (1653) identify the ‘long u’ in muse, tune, lute, dure (endure), mute, view, lieu, with French u, that is, of course (ȳ), and Wallis states that some also pronounce eu or iu. This would imply that there were two pronunciations, a simple (ȳ) and a diphthongized (iy). Price also (1688) suggests a diphthongal pronunciation in muse, refuse, etc., ‘as if it were composed of iw.’ On the other hand, Wilkins (1688) says that Englishmen cannot pronounce French, or, as he calls it, ‘whistling u,’ since to them, as ‘to all nations among whom it is not used, it is of so laborious and difficult pronunciation that I shall not proceed further to any explication of it.’ Wilkins transliterates ‘communion’ as (komiūnion). Apparently, then, by this time there were two old-fashioned types of pronunciation of this sound—(iȳ and ȳ), and the newer pronunciations (iū and ū). These sounds represented, not only M.E. ȳ, but also M.E. eu, as in (diȳ), ‘dew,’ M.E. deu; ([k]niȳ), ‘knew,’ M.E. kneu; (bliȳ), ‘blue,’ M.E. bleu, etc. It seems probable that the (ȳ) lost its front quality in the third quarter of the seventeenth century, so that the two types were (bliu), corresponding to earlier (bliȳ), and (blū), corresponding to earlier (blȳ). At the present day, in the Standard language, we have on the one hand (blū), ‘blue,’ (þrū), ‘threw,’ (rūl), ‘rule,’ etc., and on the other (tjūzdi), ‘tuesday,’ (mjūz), ‘muse,’ (fjū), ‘few,’ (stjūpid), also (stjupid), ‘stupid,’ (djūk), ‘duke,’ etc., corresponding to sixteenth-century (iȳ) and (ȳ) respectively. In dialectal speech different types often exist from those used in the Standard, and (dūk) from (dȳk), (stūpid) from (stȳpid), (tūzdi) from (tȳzdei), (nū), ‘new,’ from (nȳ), are quite common. Again, provincial (ríūl), ‘rule,’ (blíū), and (bljū), ‘blue,’ (fríūt), ‘fruit,’ etc., also exist.

Cure is now variously (kjūə, kjuə, kjɔə, and kjɔ̄), or, in those dialects where the r is preserved, (kjūr) or (kjuər). Wallis indicates the pronunciation (kȳr), and Cooper, already, (kiuər). The only word which preserves O.E. (Saxon) ȳ in the Standard dialect is bruise (brūz), where the ui is actually a Southern M.E. spelling for ȳ.

The dialects of Devonshire and Somerset seem still to preserve a sound approximating to the M.E. and sixteenth-century (ȳ) to the present day.

The Middle English Diphthongs.

M.E. ai and ei.—These diphthongs were often confused in Late M.E., to judge by the spelling. The Welsh authorities of the sixteenth century make no distinction. The Hymn to the Virgin writes ai, ae, ay in away, awae, kae, agaynst, and ei only in ddey, ddei. Salesbury transliterates both sounds by ai, ay—vain = ‘vein’ and ‘vain’; nayl = ‘nail.’ Salesbury uses ei for the new diphthong M.E. EI, AI from old (ī).

On the other hand, Palsgrave (1530) distinguishes between (ɛi) in obey, grey, in which ‘e shall have his distinct sound,’ and (ai) in rayne, ‘rain,’ payn, ‘pain,’ fayne, ‘fain,’ etc., in which ‘a is sounded distinctly, and i shortly and confusedly.’ Smith (1568) says the distinction between the two is very slight, but admits (ϵi) in feint, deinte, peint, fein (verb). He says that certain affected women, who wish to appear to speak ‘more urbanely,’ pronounce (ei) or (ϵi) not only in words where it is written, but also in words with ai, as in dai, wai, mai, tail, fail, pain, claim, plai, arai, etc. Of these, wai, ‘way,’ should, from the etymological point of view, have (ϵi). Smith says the first element is short among ‘urbane’ speakers, but that country folks pronounce it long, ‘with an odious kind of sound, fat and greasy to excess,’ saying daai, paai, etc.

These remarks surely mean that the distinction between ai and ei no longer existed, except, perhaps, artificially, through the influence of the spelling. Apparently Smith himself pronounced (ai) with the first element very short and slightly fronted; old-fashioned people and country-folk said (ai) with a full back vowel in the first element, and affected persons and ‘silly women,’ or ‘mopseys,’ as they were called, (æi) or even (ϵi), thus anticipating the fashionable pronunciation of a later day. There can be no doubt that the pronunciation of the affected persons was gaining the day, for Hart, in 1569, recognises no diphthong at all, but gives pre, we, se, etc., for ‘pray,’ ‘way,’ ‘say.’ Gill (1629) strongly condemns ‘mopseys’ in general, and Hart in particular, and disapproves of (mɛ̄dz) for (maids), ‘maids,’ and (plɛ̄) for (plai). Butler (1623) records with disapproval the pronunciation (ɛ̄) in may, nay, play, pray, say, stay, fray, slay, pay, bailey, travail. Wallis and Wilkins both describe a diphthong that must be intended for (æi). Price (1668) admits a diphthong (æi) in a good many words with ai and ey, but a single vowel (ɛ̄) apparently in many others. Cooper (1685) admits a diphthong in a few words—brain, eight, frail—otherwise ai, ay for him has the sound of contemporary a, that is, (æ) or (ϵ), and he gives the following words as pairs containing the same vowel, long and short respectively: sailsell, saintsent, tailtell, tainttent, which must imply (ɛ̄) in (sɛ̄l), ‘sail,’ etc.

The result of these somewhat contradictory accounts seems to be that M.E. ei, ai were early (in the sixteenth century) levelled under one sound in the best speech, probably (ai). The diphthongal character was lost in some dialects, retained in others, though whether these were class dialects, or associated with a geographical area, we cannot say. The Standard language tended more and more to front and raise the first element in those cases where diphthongal pronunciation remained, and by the end of the seventeenth century the monophthongal pronunciation (ǣ), or among the younger generation (ɛ̄), was fully established, so that the sound was levelled under that of M.E. ā, and henceforth shared the same development, being gradually tensened to (ē), which was subsequently diphthongized again to (ei) or (ɛi) in the nineteenth century.

Many dialects retain to the present day the M.E. vowel (ai) recorded as that of country folks in the seventeenth century, in words like (tail, pail), ‘tail,’ ‘pail,’ etc.

Early Modern English au.—This sound existed in the M.E. (AU) BECOMES (Ɔ̄) sixteenth century in words of several classes. They were mostly inherited from M.E., and to this there is only one possible exception. The (au) diphthongs, which are certainly of M.E. origin, occurred in the following conditions:

1. M.E. au or aw from O.E. -ag-: M.E. sāwe, ‘saw,’ O.E. sagu; M.E. drawen, ‘draw,’ O.E. dragan; from O.E. -aw-: M.E. clawe, ‘claw,’ O.E. clawu; O.E. -ah-: M.E. laughen, O.E. hlahhan.

2. M.E. au from Anglo-Fr. au: cause, ‘cause.’

3. In the combination original an followed by another consonant in words of Anglo-Fr. or Fr. origin: daunger, ‘danger’; aungel, ‘angel’; haunt, jaundice, etc.

(au) further occurred in stressed syllables where a was followed by l in words both of English and French origin: all, sixteenth-century (aul), fall, sixteenth-century (faul), call, sixteenth-century (kaul). According to Sweet (H.E.S., 784), this diphthong was developed in the Early Modern period.

The history of this (au) from the sixteenth century onwards is clear. The diphthong persisted throughout the century, but towards the end, the pronunciation (ɔ̄)—i.e., low-back-tense-round—or something very like it, appears to be already established. The process of change must have been: the first element was rounded through the influence of the (ū), giving (ɔ̄u), then the second element was absorbed, and the sound was monophthongized to (ɔ̄) and tensened to (ɔ̄), its present form. From the seventeenth century onwards (ɔ̄) is the only representative of the old (au).

Sixteenth-century examples are (baul, haul, waul, faul, kaul, hau, lauful, strau, mau, tʃauns, graunt, džaundis, launs), etc. = ball, hall, wall, fall, call, haw, lawful, straw, maw, chance, grant, jaundice, lance. The (ɔ̄u) stage is occasionally recorded in the seventeenth century, but, presumably, did not last long. In that century most of these words are recorded with (ɔ̄), but occasionally, apparently, with (ɔu), written ou by Cooper and oou by Gill, which probably represents the intermediate stage.

Of the words mentioned above with (au) before n, however, only jaundice exists with (ɔ̄) in the Standard English of the present day, and many speakers, including the present writer, pronounce (dzāndis) here with (ā), as in all the other words in the list with a nasal.

In several other words of this group we have doublets in the polite pronunciation of to-day—e.g., (hɔ̄nʃ) and (hānʃ), ‘haunch’; (lɔ̄nʃ) and (lānʃ), as well as (lænʃ), ‘launch’; (vɔ̄nt) and (vānt), ‘vaunt’; (lɔ̄ndri) and (lāndri), ‘laundry ‘; (hɔ̄nt) and (hānt), ‘haunt’; also in the name Saunders or Sanders, which is pronounced according to the taste or traditions of its owner (sɔ̄ndəz) or (sāndəz). Dance is pronounced both (dāns) and (dæns), (dɔ̄ns) having disappeared; lance = (lāns) or (læns), but there is no (lɔ̄ns), and the name Launcelot is never (lɔ̄nsilot), only (lānsilot) or (lænsilot).

The first point to be clear about is that the pronunciation (ɔ̄) in any of these words represents an older (au). But (au) or its descendant (ɔ̄) were not the only forms in use in the seventeenth century. Side by side with these we find also doublets with (æ) which are sometimes given by the same authorities as alternatives to the (ɔ̄) pronunciation. Thus we find (dænt, flænt, hænt, džænt, tænt) = daunt, flaunt, haunt, jaunt, taunt. These would appear to be the ancestors of the modern forms with (ā). They DEVELOPMENT OF M.E. -(AN)- AND -(AL)- gave rise to two types—one which retained (æ), another in which it was lengthened to (ǣ). The short forms remain, and correspond to the present-day (dæns, læsnʃ), etc.; the long forms develop (ā) in the late eighteenth century, and are therefore the direct ancestors of (lānʃ, lāndri), etc.

The existence of the types (lænʃ), (lɔ̄nʃ) side by side in the seventeenth century shows that by the side of (launʃ), etc., which gave rise to the latter, forms such as (lanʃ), the ancestor of the former, must have existed, although not recorded, in the sixteenth century. This proves that in M.E. the Anglo-French combination -an- before a consonant was not universally diphthongized to (aun), but that a type -(an)- also existed. This probability is also suggested by the fluctuation of M.E. spelling, which writes both haunten and hanten. Non-diphthongized forms also existed of the -al- combinations. Present-day (kāf), ‘calf,’ (kām), ‘calm,’ (kwām), ‘qualm,’ (sām), ‘psalm,’ (hāf), ‘half,’ etc., are from eighteenth-century (kǣf), seventeenth-century (kæf), sixteenth-century (kalf and kaf), and so on with the others. The pronunciation (kwɔ̄m), which is sometimes heard, of course represents a doublet (kwaulm). Scotch (hɔ̄f), etc., is the representative of sixteenth-century (haulf).

Present-day English has (lāftə, drāft) by the side of (tɔ̄t, fɔ̄t), ‘laughter, draught, taught, aught.’ Here, again, we have the survivals of two distinct types: (lāftə), etc., comes from eighteenth-century (lǣftər), from (læftər), from (lafter). This may well be a M.E. treatment of (h), in which case there would be no diphthonging. Those speakers, on the other hand, who said (lahter) developed the form (lau[h]ter), which is, indeed, recorded for the sixteenth century, together with its descendant (lɔ̄[h]ter) later on. This is the form apparently represented by our traditional spelling. This type still survives in Scotch, (tɔ̄t) is the normal development of M.E. tāhte, and in this word it would seem that no doublet with (f) survives.

M.E. ou.—The vowel in thought, brought, daughter, etc., which represents M.E. o, with a glide vowel developed before h, as in the case of M.E. -ah-, has apparently passed through an (ōu) stage, at which point it must have been levelled with the earlier (au), or the series may have been (ou) with slack o, (ō) with long slack o after absorption of u, and the levelling of such a long vowel with (ɔ̄) is a natural tendency.

The Consonants in the Modern Period.

On the whole, but little change has taken place in the pronunciation of the consonants since the sixteenth century. There are, however, a few points which deserve notice.

The symbols -gh- medially or finally were pronounced, according to the nature of the preceding vowel, as a front or back open voiceless consonant (h). That this had in some dialects a lip modification, when back, is evident from the fact that in a large number of words in Standard English it has become pure (f). In words where it represented a Front open consonant, and in a few where it was Back, (h) remained, apparently with a very slight consonantal friction, well into the seventeenth century, in the pronunciation of some speakers. It seems probable that in most words with back (h) two types of pronunciation existed in the sixteenth century—(lafter) and (lahter), CHANGES IN CONSONANT SOUNDS (boft) and (boht), ‘laughter,’ ‘bought,’ etc. At any rate, both of these types are proved to have existed in the above words and in many others, while the evidence of the Modern dialects, taken together with the Standard language, would greatly extend the list. Of course, no (u) glide was developed in the (f) types, and there are consequently no examples of the combination (-ɔ̄f-) in these words, unless, indeed, it exists in some of the popular dialects, in which case it is the result of a blending of two types—the vowel of one and the consonant of the other.

Initial kn-, gn-.—The combination -kn- retained the initial stop, at any rate until the seventeenth century. From the testimony of the authorities it seems probable that n was unvoiced in this position, and the (k) lost. Cooper says that knave is pronounced like hnave, which seems to imply a voiceless n. In the late seventeenth and early eighteenth centuries the authorities are at variance as to the pronunciation of gn-, Jones making it ordinary (voiced) n, while Lediard describes voiceless n. Possibly gn- and kn- had both been levelled under the latter sound, in which case we might conclude that in the early eighteenth century the voiceless pronunciation still existed, while the new voiced n was coming in.

Initial wr-.—The w was still heard down to the beginning of the eighteenth century. It still remains in this position in certain Scotch dialects, as (v)—e.g., vrīt, ‘write,’ in Aberdeenshire.

Loss of r.—This is, perhaps, one of the most considerable changes that has taken place in recent English, especially the Standard dialect. r is lost medially before consonants, and finally unless the next word in the breath-group begins with a vowel. With the loss of r certain modifications have occurred in the preceding vowels: (1) Development of vowel murmur, as in (faiə, biəd); (2) the levelling of several distinct vowels under (ʌ̄), as in (bʌ̄d, wʌ̄d, lʌ̄n, wʌ̄m, hʌ̄d), or under (ɔ̄), as in (hɔ̄d, mɔ̄, pjɔ̄).


CHAPTER XV

THE STUDY OF PRESENT-DAY ENGLISH

Although it has been found convenient, as a matter of systematic arrangement, to reserve this subject until the end of the present work, it is nevertheless strongly to be recommended that, in teaching, the study of actual living English should serve as the starting-point of, and as the preparation for, the historical study of our language.

The reason for this must have become apparent from the general tenor of this book. The first preparation for a competent study of the history of a language is some training in phonetics, and for this the native spoken language must serve as a basis. The first lessons in accurate observation and analysis of speech sounds must be learned, as has been repeatedly pointed out, from one’s own speech, and that of one’s associates.

From the study of the sounds of his own language, the student will naturally proceed to examine the structure, the accidence, and syntax of the spoken form of English. The methods of such an investigation have been exemplified in Mr. Sweet’s Primer of Spoken English, 1900, and this admirable work may serve as a model to the teacher who conducts a class in the subject, though it must naturally be borne in mind that just as Mr. Sweet has described his own pronunciation, so the student must learn to observe and describe his own, noting the points of agreement and of difference between his own speech habits and those of his associates, and between that set forth in the Primer.

When at least some knowledge of the facts of contemporary English has been gained, the next step is to inquire how they arose; and to answer this question involves an inquiry into the earlier forms of our language. For this, one trained to observe the facts of actually existing speech has the best kind of preparation. He has been brought face to face with the realities of language in its spoken form; he has learnt to recognise that linguistic study is primarily concerned with what is uttered and heard; he has acquired to some extent the power of understanding what is meant by sound change; he has found from observation that various factors are at work in modifying the speech of the individual; he knows something of analogy; he has seen that speech habits vary from individual to individual, and from community to community. Thus, from a systematic and intelligent study of the spoken language, the beginner has been made familiar with many of the facts and general principles which it is essential to know and understand in order to grasp the vital points of linguistic development.

The Relation of Written and Spoken English.

The first ‘vulgar error’ which it is necessary to dispel is the belief that good speakers, in ordinary conversation, merely reproduce the language of books, and that the Spoken is based upon the Literary language.

The language of conversation has an independent life, quite apart from the written forms of speech. Literature, SPEAKING AND WRITING among a highly-educated community, especially one whose ideas and experiences are drawn more from books than from life, undoubtedly influences the Spoken language, but it is not the main source of this. The source of Spoken English is, mainly and primarily, direct tradition of utterance, passed on from one generation to another. The sources of the language of literature are twofold: first, literary tradition, and secondly, though equally important, the spoken language of the period. The term Spoken English has been used in the present case to cover all the various forms of the language spoken throughout the country; the term Written Language, to cover at once the language of literature proper, and the humbler attempts of ordinary speakers to record their ideas in writing instead of in speech sounds—to use, that is, symbols of a different order to represent what is already a group of symbols.

It will be convenient, for purposes of contrast, to select one type of Written English on one hand, and of Spoken English on the other. For the former we take what we may call the Literary English proper: that form of the written language which is regulated by tradition, which is deliberate, self-conscious, and artistic. For the latter we take what may be called Standard Spoken English, which we have often referred to by this name in earlier chapters of this book.

There is what the present writer believes to be an unfortunate habit among some authorities on linguistic subjects, of bracketing Literary and Standard Spoken English together, under the single name Literary English, thereby confusing two distinct phenomena, and suggesting the very fallacy which it is so important to avoid, namely, that this form of the spoken language is derived from, or a reproduction of, the language of literature. The idea that those speakers of English who do not speak what is technically known as a Dialect, in the special sense of the term, are reproducing, or attempting to reproduce, in their speech the language of books is fundamentally erroneous. This would be possible, though not desirable, as regards style and vocabulary; it is impossible in the domain of pronunciation. To speak of the sounds of Literary English is an absurdity, since what is written has no sounds until it is uttered, and then it naturally is pronounced according to the speech habits of the particular reader. When Dr. Wright, in the English Dialect Gr., speaks of the pronunciation of ‘Literary English,’ he means, of course, Standard Spoken English. What we have called Standard English, but what may also be called Polite English, or, with certain qualifications, simply Good English, is as much a reality as the dialect of West Somerset or of Windhill; it has had a normal and natural growth from a particular form of fifteenth-century English, and although it has, in the course of time, incorporated fresh elements from the outside, and discarded others that were once part and parcel of it, its history can be traced, as we have attempted to show in the former chapter, with considerable certainty for more than 300 years. Standard English, it is true, is no longer a regional dialect; it is emphatically a class dialect, which is fast absorbing other forms of Spoken English. Present-day Standard English, as we have already seen, springs originally from the same source as the literary dialect—that is, from the London dialect of the fifteenth century; and just as this, in its RELATION OF LITERARY TO SPOKEN ENGLISH written form, at a much earlier date, gained universal currency in writings, so the former is now gradually but surely gaining ground among all classes and in all areas. What the printing press did long ago for the written form, modern means of locomotion are doing to-day for the spoken. We shall return later to the important question of ‘good’ and ‘bad’ in speech; in the meantime, it may be pointed out that the Standard dialect of English is to some extent more artificial than other forms of Spoken English, in that it is more subject to fashion, and, it may perhaps be admitted, more shaped, in any given age, by a deliberate selective and eliminating process. What, then, is the relation of this form of Spoken English to the language of Literature?

Both, as has been said, are sprung originally from the same source; they have developed differently by virtue of the different conditions under which they severally exist. One great and obvious external difference between Written and Spoken English is that, whereas the spelling of the former is fixed, and no longer expresses the variations of sound which exist in different areas, and arise in different ages, the spoken form is for ever undergoing changes in pronunciation, with the passage of time and the spread of this dialect among all sections of the population. The spelling of Literary English, then, no longer expresses, even approximately, the facts of actual utterance, as they exist in Standard Spoken English, in its different varieties.

But the differences between Written and Spoken English are deeper than those produced merely by a pronunciation which has far outstripped its symbolical expression, and include also differences of style, of idiom, of choice of words, and grammatical forms.

The language of literature, in all these respects, is always slightly more archaic than the uttered speech of the same period; certain words and expressions are avoided in writing a serious prose, because they are felt to be too familiar—too closely associated with the commonplaces and vulgarities of everyday existence; others, on the other hand, find no place in the Spoken language, because they seem to savour of pomposity or bookishness.

But literary style changes from age to age. To a certain extent each generation has its own style. Matthew Arnold appears to fail in perfect critical insight when he points to a noble passage from Dryden’s Preface to his translation of the Æneid, and remarks that it is ‘such a prose as we would all gladly use if we only knew how.’ This is neither adequate as an appreciation of Dryden, nor is it strictly true. Only in very special circumstances, and as an exercise in imitation, would a writer of the present day ‘gladly use’ the prose of the seventeenth century. Herein, indeed, lies the heart of the whole matter. The literary language is kept living and flexible only by a close relation with the colloquial speech of the age. A purely literary tradition, however splendid, will not suffice for the style of a later period. A literary tradition alone, deprived of the living spirit which informs the great works that created the tradition, is a lifeless thing. The breath of life comes into literary form from the living spoken language, as it comes into literature itself from touch with life. Thus, while great prose owes much to tradition, it owes still more to the racy speech of the age in which it is produced. The best prose is never entirely remote in THE LIFE-BLOOD OF LITERATURE form from the best corresponding conversational style of the period. A robust, intense style glows with emotion, and pulsates with passion; a calm and restrained prose must yet be animated with an undercurrent of strenuous thought or genuine feeling. If these be lacking, the most accomplished reproduction of an old literary model is stiff and uninteresting.

The impression made by fine prose of any age, and not infrequently also by verse, of the less artificial and elaborate kind, is that the author writes very much as he would speak, if he were conveying the same ideas by word of mouth. This is felt strongly in reading Chaucer’s Canterbury Tales, in those passages where the felicitousness and competence of expression reaches its highest point; it is felt in reading Latimer’s Sermons; in nearly all of Dryden’s critical prose; in the Letters of Horace Walpole and of Gray; in Swift, in Goldsmith, and in Sheridan.

It is this quality of vitality, which springs from a mastery of the best spoken form of English of his age, that compels our admiration in the prose of Dryden; but what we should ‘gladly use’ is not his precise form, which is no longer a living vehicle of thought and feeling, but a prose which should combine the elements of literary tradition on the one hand, with those of contemporary colloquial speech on the other, in that just proportion, and with that subtle blending, which is the secret of great writers in all ages. No writer can express himself adequately in a language which is not his own; the thoughts and emotions of one age cannot be conveyed in a style which is outworn; and this has come about when the relation between the language of literature and that of everyday life is severed.

It would probably be a fruitful investigation to trace the connection between the prose style of the seventeenth and eighteenth centuries and that of the closest reproduction of the conversational style of the corresponding period which we possess—that is, the language of the Comic Drama.

The Spoken Language.

One of the most striking features of living, uttered speech is its adaptability. Standard English is not fixed and rigid in form; in the same period, and in the mouth of the same speaker, it is not invariable under all conditions, and in every kind of company. The actual sounds employed, the speed of utterance, the intonation, the sentence structure, the choice of vocabulary, are all variable according to the requirements of the moment. The speaker adapts his speech, both in public oration, and in private conversation, to suit his audience. This modification of the language in its different elements, may be deliberate, but for the most part is unconscious and instinctive.

In public speaking, the manner of the discourse of an accomplished and practised orator is determined to a great extent by the size of the audience; but also by the speaker’s estimate of their mental calibre, no less than by his own. Upon this power of ‘getting into touch’ with his hearers, on the part of the speaker, the success and effectiveness of an academic lecture, a political harangue, or an after-dinner speech will largely depend. There is room for an investigation into the variations of style, ADAPTABILITY OF THE SPOKEN LANGUAGE vocabulary, idiom, and syntax of the same speaker, according to the size, intellectual quality, and general temper of his audience.

Public oratory is that form of the Spoken language which comes nearest to the language of literature in style. But if this form of uttered language is liable to modification in the manner indicated, the private speech of ordinary conversation is no less sensitive to the modifying influences of social atmosphere. There is room for a vast amount of variability in the colloquial speech of the same individual, according to the company in which he is placed. Phraseology, vocabulary, even pronunciation, tend, each and all, to adapt themselves to the personality and attainments of the person addressed. The manner of speech may be perfectly natural, or it may become stilted, pompous, flippant, archaic, or slangy, accordingly as the real or fancied personality of the hearer excites reverence, trepidation, confidence, affection, or contempt in the mind of the speaker. The disparity which provokes such departure from the normal colloquial style, may be of the most varied kind: it may consist in difference of rank, of official status, age, intellectual or moral worth, or in worldly success, all of which affect different minds in different ways.

In some cases convention, as it were, strikes the keynote, by prescribing by what title certain personages shall be addressed, but the rest is left to the instinct or intuition of the speaker. Thus, by a convention which will probably never change, the Deity, in both private and public devotions, is invariably addressed in the second person singular; and in this solitary case the pronoun of that person is preserved, which is otherwise completely obsolete in Standard English, except among members of the Society of Friends.

There can be no doubt that the best speaker, whether in private or public, is he, the form of whose discourse instinctively shapes itself to the requirements of the moment, without any apparent effort or deliberation.

For there is a limit beyond which adaptiveness cannot go, without awakening resentment or uneasiness in the hearers, or, what is perhaps worse, without imperilling the vividness and sense of reality in the expression; and this limit is reached very soon after the modification of form, or choice of verbiage becomes self-conscious and deliberate. If a speaker reacts too much to his environment—to borrow a phrase from the vocabulary of Biology—if he is either overawed by a sense of the superiority of those to whom he speaks, or too deeply conscious of the reverse quality, all naturalness of speech is at an end. For in one case a speaker will speak too carefully and pedantically: he will mince in his pronunciation, and, worst of all, perhaps tend to obsequiousness; in the other, a sense of self-importance may bloat his diction to pomposity, and convey the feeling that he is trying hard to be worthy of himself. Or, again, by a too familiar and undignified discourse, he may make his hearers feel that by an infinite condescension he is coming down from an immeasurable height to their level, and perhaps sinking below it. In both cases the speaker may fall back upon set phrases devoid of character. Thus the right and proper adaptation of spoken language cannot be carried out on any preconceived principle, but must spring from a sympathetic and STEREOTYPED VERBIAGE humane insight into the personality of those to whom we speak, a nice appreciation of the psychological conditions of the moment. If a speaker would sway his audience to his own mood, or instil his own opinions into their minds, if he would ‘carry them with him,’ as the phrase runs, he must first lay his finger upon the pulse of their temper and of their prejudices. The speaker himself must barely perceive the process of adaptation, the hearers not at all; they are merely conscious that the form in which the ideas are clothed is entirely suitable and convincing.

Lifeless Forms of English.

A living form of speech is one which expresses real ideas and feelings and genuine convictions in a form suited to the audience and the occasion, springing from the mind of the speaker in the process of his thought, and revealing something at least of his personality. In order to arrest attention and compel interest, an utterance, whether it be a public oration or familiar discourse, must contain something more than the obvious truisms of a proposition in Euclid; the style in which the thoughts are clothed must be personal to the speaker, and not the mere repetition of set phrases. The essentials of living utterance are, then, reality of conviction, and individuality of form and phrase. Both of these qualities are very often found to a remarkable degree in quite uncultivated, and even in ‘illiterate,’ speakers. From these realities of speech life, we now turn aside for a short space, to consider a dreary linguistic waste of crystallized phrases, lifeless forms devoid of movement or feeling, peopled only with the ghosts of ideas, and the spectral shadows of human desires.

There are many types of unreal, lifeless English; they range from the terrible phrases of ‘Commercial English,’ such as ‘Your esteemed favour of even date to hand,’ through those unconvincing fossils of language which help to fill space in the daily paper—‘The greatest consternation prevailed when the news of the disaster reached the city,’ or the curious jargon known as ‘Committee English’—‘Your committee beg to report that while fully recognising the importance of the subject of ——, they consider that, under the circumstances, it is undesirable to take any further steps in the matter for the present’—up to the language of public legal documents and of high officialdom. All these lifeless forms of English have at least this in common: they consist largely of cut-and-dried phrases pieced together. In these phrases, whether they be uttered or written, there lurks no human emotion, no intensity of thought; they reveal nothing of the state of mind of him who uses them; they kindle no hope or enthusiasm in the hearer. The cheap verbiage of the penny-a-liner is generally the cloak of his incapacity to express anything; the stereotyped phrases of the fluent committee debater, or of the official generally, are devices for politely shelving inconvenient questions, or are intended to guard the speaker from identifying himself, or his office, too intimately and irrevocably, with any particular line of thought or action. The characteristic effect of a diction of set expressions artfully tagged together, whether this be the result of incompetence, as in the case of a bad writer, or of design, as in that of a wary and experienced official, is that it is singularly lacking in interest VUGARITY OF CONVENTIONAL PHRASES or power of convincing those to whom it is addressed. Thus the historian of the Police Court does not quicken our pulses by a single beat by his account of ‘a young lady of prepossessing appearance, fashionably attired,’ etc. If a body of starving men petition Parliament to relieve their necessities, it neither appeases their hunger, nor calms their anxiety, to be told that their circumstances ‘will receive the careful consideration of the Government.’

Clothed in the language of conventional set phrase, the noblest thoughts and loftiest aspirations are robbed of their grandeur and become commonplace; events of the greatest solemnity and moment, or the actions of heroes, shrink to the insignificance of a meeting of directors; while what is trite or vulgar, in feeling, or in ideas, simply vanishes altogether amid the meaningless verbiage.

Distressing as the habit is of using a series of stereotyped expressions, even in formal deliverances on public bodies, or in the written forms in journalism, it must be recognised that it is very much worse to do so in private intercourse, either in conversation or in correspondence. It is felt that to speak ‘Committee English’ in private is an offence which can only arise, either, from ill-breeding, or from ignorance of the proper forms of polite Spoken English. ‘Proverbial expressions and trite sayings,’ says Lord Chesterfield, ‘are the flowers of the rhetoric of a vulgar man.’ Whatever be the cause which induces a speaker to mask his real feelings and views in this lifeless form of language, the result is fatal to a satisfactory understanding. The sense of sincerity, ease, and reality vanishes, and an uncomfortable atmosphere of uncertainty, if not of absolute distrust, is created. There can be no doubt that for those who have not habitually heard good, racy, expressive Polite English spoken from childhood, this is a most necessary side of English study from a purely practical point of view. Unfortunately, it is almost universally supposed to be enough to acquire a fairly good knowledge of the written language, and the differences between good Written and good Spoken English are completely ignored, not only in primary schools, but also in the curriculum for the training of teachers.

The art of speaking English so as to be ‘familiar, but by no means vulgar,’ is apparently supposed to be the common heritage of the primary teacher. This is, however, as far as possible from being the case. It is perfectly true that the only way of learning to speak any dialect readily and fluently, whether it be good English or good French, is to hear it and use it so frequently that it becomes instinctive. At the same time, much help in the direction of observation can be given, and should be given systematically. Now, many persons in this country, who are otherwise highly educated, fail signally in possessing a command of easy, natural, Polite Spoken English. The reason for this is that they have not grown up in circles where this kind of English is current, neither have they had their attention directed to its characteristics. The result is they have the choice between the English of books or of set phrases on the one hand, or on the other, a form more or less ‘incorrect’ or ‘provincial,’ perhaps, but nevertheless a living form, which they have been carefully taught to avoid.

The fact is that the native form of Spoken English is eliminated by training, but no colloquial form is put in its THE CONCEPTION OF A STANDARD OF SPEECH place.

The importance of the study of Spoken English has been constantly emphasized in the foregoing pages as a necessary preparation for the historical study of the language, and as a starting-point of phonetic training. From this point of view, the student’s own natural speech forms the proper basis of study, and so long as that inquiry is confined to the above-mentioned limits, no question of ‘Right’ or ‘Wrong’ arises—merely that of what actually occurs in the speech of a given individual or group of individuals. But from the practical, as contrasted with the purely historical and scientific, standpoint the power of writing and speaking ‘correct’ English cannot be disregarded in any complete scheme of education, and it is now suggested that it is quite as necessary to speak well as to write well. In the study of Spoken English, from the practical point of view, three main sides of the subject must be dealt with: Pronunciation, Vocabulary, and the choice of Idiom.

Standards of Good or Bad Spoken English.

It has been made abundantly clear in the course of the present volume that there is no absolute standard of ‘correctness’ in language beyond that established by the habitual usage of a given community. Such a standard, as has been said, holds good for that community at a given moment. But as speech habit changes, so ideas of what is ‘right’ and ‘wrong’ have also to be readjusted. From this point of view, which is the purely scientific one, there is no question of degrees of worthiness between different dialects; they are each and all regarded merely as varying phases of linguistic development—the facts of each and all equally deserve attention. We now pass to examine a little more closely a different view of language, one which definitely holds that of the numerous forms of English, one is pre-eminently Good English, the best and most polite among the dialects.

It has been said in an earlier chapter (cf. pp. 22-25) that it is possible to over-estimate the degree of uniformity with which Standard English is spoken throughout the country, and it should be remembered that a form of language which is disseminated over so wide a geographical area and among such divers classes must inevitably undergo a certain degree of differentiation. The checks which exist upon the tendency to differentiate Standard English, and the forces which make possible so large a degree of uniformity as undoubtedly exists, have already been discussed (cf. pp. 99-105). It is perhaps not strange that the very phrase Standard English should arouse antagonism in minds which, possibly through no fault of the individual, are prejudiced by being insufficiently informed.

It is perhaps said, ‘You admit a considerable amount of differentiation in your so-called Standard English, and yet you adhere to the conception of a Standard. How is this logical?’ The reply to this objection is, that the distinctions between the different forms of Standard English are very slight, almost imperceptible, indeed, to any but the most alert and practised observer, and that they shrink to a negligible quantity compared with the differences between out-and-out ‘Vulgarism’ on the one hand, or provincial—that is, regional—dialects on PRONUNCIATION THE TEST OF ‘CORRECTNESS’ the other.

In Standard English, as with all other forms of speech, a certain degree of divergence is possible, without such divergence being felt as constituting a different dialect. Of a dozen speakers of Standard English, each may possess slight differences of utterance, or phraseology, and yet none feel that the speech of any of the others, even where it differs from his own, verges towards Vulgarism or ‘Dialect’ in the special sense.

The most noteworthy criterion of Good English, or Standard English, is pronunciation. In this respect there are two main points to be observed—the actual sounds employed and the proper distribution of those sounds; that is, the use of them in the right words. The fact that a certain group of sounds, and those sounds only (subject to the slight divergences already mentioned), and, further, a certain distribution of those sounds, is accepted in the polite usage is the result of convention. The fundamental reason of that convention is that certain pronunciations are associated by long habit with a cultivated mind, liberal education, refined taste, and good breeding generally; other pronunciations are associated with the reverse qualities of mind and manners. The former mode of pronunciation is held to be an indication of the possession of the politer education. If it be asked where this superior form of English is heard, it may be answered, that on the whole, it is the speech in vogue at the Court, in the Church, at the Bar, at the older Universities, and at the great Public Schools. The English of the stage is also a form of Standard English, but it differs from the English of good society, partly in being more archaic, partly also in being marred by certain artificialities and affectations of pronunciation. That a standard form of English has been in existence, sedulously cultivated, and jealously (if often foolishly) treasured, for the last 350 years at least, no one who has studied the authorities upon English Pronunciation, from the middle of the sixteenth century downwards, quoted in the preceding chapter, can have any doubt whatever.

At the present time it will not be denied that to inculcate the speaking of correct English is the chief solicitude of a very large number of persons engaged in Primary and Secondary Education in this country. Those whose business it is to teach, who are to become public speakers, or who wish to enter upon public life, or affairs of any kind, undoubtedly find it convenient to get rid of whatever native ‘vulgarisms’ or dialectal peculiarities their speech contains, and to attempt to approximate their Spoken English to that standard form which is no longer confined to a single province, or to a particular social class.

In the face of these facts it cannot be thought presumptuous to insist upon the existence of a recognised standard of English speech, to endeavour to arrive at some clear ideas as to its characteristics, and to indicate a reasonable way of regarding it.

In such an inquiry the main things to be avoided are, on the one hand, tolerating too great slackness and slovenliness, which is the fallacy of those who incline to reject the whole conception of a standard of speech, and on the other the pedantic insistence upon precious and artificial forms of language; the setting up, in fact, of a false standard of perfection, which is the prevailing sin of those THE ELEMENT OF ARTIFICIALITY who are over-anxious to speak ‘correctly.’

It has been said, that owing to social circumstances, a certain type of English speech is regarded as an evidence of cultivation and refinement, and this in itself would constitute a strong claim for this form of English to be considered as worthy of attention; but it might further be urged that Standard English has an absolute superiority over any other dialect in the high degree of acoustic distinctness which it possesses, compared with the provincial or vulgar forms of English. This quality makes it eminently suitable for public speaking.

To what Extent Standard English is Artificial.

In a perfectly natural, unconventional, and popular form of speech, such as we may find in many of the remote provincial dialects of this country, the speakers do not consider the question of ‘correctness’ or the reverse. They speak the dialect as it was transmitted to them, without inquiring whether one of two variants which may exist within the dialect, in certain cases, is ‘better’ than the other.

In fact, ordinary dialect speakers have no standard of speech, or none, at least, determined by any canons of taste, or what is called ‘good form.’ Such is the position of all primitive languages, of all such as are not the vehicles of culture, or of such, as by the force of social conditions, have become, as it were, backwaters of the great stream of national speech. This subordinate position of the provincial dialects is the inevitable result of the rise of one immensely predominant form of language, as that of the official classes, and of the most cultivated portion of the community. When one dialect obtains the dignity of becoming the channel of all that is worthiest in the national literature and the national civilization, the other less favoured dialects shrink into obscurity and insignificance. The latter preserve, however, this advantage, considered as types of linguistic development, that the primitive conditions under which language exists, and changes, are far more faithfully represented in them than in the cultivated dialect. For it is a characteristic, and necessarily so, of a standard dialect, that the question of what is ‘Right’ or ‘Wrong,’ ‘Correct’ or ‘Incorrect,’ ‘Good Form’ or ‘Bad Form,’ ‘Polite’ or ‘Vulgar,’ should be raised.

From the moment that such conceptions as these are introduced, a certain element of artificiality arises in that form of language which is affected by them. This element of artificiality, however, lies, as a rule, not in the actual forms or phrases themselves, nor in the mode of their development, but simply in the fact that a more or less deliberate choice is exercised by the speakers in eliminating, or adopting for use this or that particular pronunciation, word, phrase, or construction. It is important to realize that the most fastidious speaker does not create new forms himself, nor deliberately carry out a sound change. Unless he is deliberately artificial, the individual merely exercises a power of selection from among speech elements, sounds, and the rest, which exist already, and which have arisen by a perfectly natural and normal process of development. Thus even in the most highly cultivated form of Standard Dialects, whether it be English or any other language, speakers cannot consciously alter the course of the natural trend of development; this goes on unperceived, SHIFTING OF THE STANDARD OF ‘CORRECTNESS’ here, as in the most barbarous and primitive form of speech. But in the Standard Language, at any given period, certain modes of speech may be definitely avoided, while others are habitually used.

The standard of what is Polite or the reverse varies from age to age, and in former chapters of this book examples of this fluctuation have been given. One factor, which determines the rejection of what was formerly held to be the best usage, is undoubtedly the spread of Standard English among various social classes, with the result that a particular pronunciation, word or phrase, loses distinction, and acquires so common a currency, that with it an association of vulgarity or lack of refinement is formed. There is in this respect an analogy between fashion in speech and other fashions or habits. They may start high up in the social scale, and be gradually imitated and adopted as signs of superiority by the lower grades of society. By the time, however, that the fashion has become firmly fixed among such classes as do not usually enjoy a reputation for refinement and distinction, it has been already discarded by those divisions of society whence it originally proceeded. In the curious turns of fashion in speech, not only is that given up which an earlier generation considered good, but what they held as vulgar is often adopted by their successors.

The differences in pronunciation which exist at a given time, between the various sections of English people who speak what we may call a variety of Standard English, consist for the most part, not of differences in the actual sounds used, but in the distribution of the sounds. It is, of course, merely a question of degree, but we must admit that such a pronunciation as that of the Cockney (raiuwai) ‘railway,’ with the triphthong (aiu), which is absolutely unknown in the best Standard English, in any word, reveals a wider dialectal difference from the received form (rɛilwɛi), than that of such a pronunciation as (dæns) instead of the (in the South) more usual (dāns), or (kɔ̄fi), ‘coffee,’ as compared with (kɔfi). Again, the Cockney sound in the unstressed syllable of ‘father’ (mid-flat-tense, instead of slack), or in that ‘boots’ (high-back-out-tense-round, instead of the full-back), are sounds which the speakers of the best English never by any chance employ—which, indeed, they would probably have considerable difficulty in reproducing. Such differences as these constitute, as it appears, not a mere Variety, but a different Dialect. On the other hand, such pronunciations as (kɔf, ɔltə, hjūmərəs, pjuə, or pjūə, kɔ̄təsi) as compared with (kɔ̄f, ɔ̄ltə, jūmərəs, pjɔ̄, kʌ̄təsi) do not constitute more than varieties, or alternative pronunciations, both of which are, at the present time, perhaps almost equally widespread among speakers of good Standard English. The existence of such alternatives seems to show a period of transition as regards the standard of pronunciation in these particular words. Probably fifty years hence fashion will have decided definitely in favour of one or other of the above types. The present writer inclines to believe that there is a slight majority of speakers of Standard English at the present time in favour of the latter group of pronunciations given above, and that in time those in the former group will disappear, as possible standard forms. There are cases where the distribution of particular sounds among a given set of words is so definitely fixed by the received LORD CHESTERFIELD ON CORRECT SPEECH usage that a deviation from such a system of distribution would be quite enough to constitute a wide difference of dialect. Thus there is not the faintest doubt that (spūn, buk, blad, klāk, dābi, vʌ̄tjū, or vʌ̄tʃū, lʌ̄n, rɔ̄þ, əmaŋ) are the received forms of these words among the best speakers, and that such pronunciations as (spun, būk [or bak], blud, klʌ̄k, dʌ̄bi, vātju, lān, rāþ, əmɔŋ) are at the present time ‘vulgarisms,’ or provincial forms.

Thus the history of a standard form of language comprises these two aspects—natural development or gradual shifting of the speech habit, and the fluctuations of fashion which determine the particular action of the selective process.

[Note.—Since the above was written, Professor Rippmann’s Sounds of Spoken English (Dent, 1906) has appeared. Students will find this book useful, and the remarks on the distribution of vowel sounds in English are particularly interesting.]

Criteria of ‘Good’ Pronunciation.

The most usual way of dealing with this question is to lay down certain definite rules as to how English ‘ought’ to be pronounced. This is the worst possible method, because it implies the existence of an absolute standard of Right and Wrong in language.

The only test of what the conventional standard of any age really is, is simply the custom of good speakers. ‘A man of fashion,’ says Lord Chesterfield—and we may give the remark a wider application—‘a man of fashion takes great care to speak very correctly and grammatically, and to pronounce properly—that is, according to the usage of the best companies.’ That is the right definition of speaking ‘correctly,’ and it can hardly be improved upon. Any system of pronunciation which is not based upon one actually in use, is merely theoretical, and therefore worthless. It is impossible to say a priori how a doubtful word may or may not be pronounced. All that a teacher of pronunciation is justified in saying is, ‘This word is pronounced in such and such a way by good speakers.’ But if he has not heard good speakers pronounce the word; if he himself is not naturally one (that is, from the time he learned to speak); or if, being a ‘good speaker,’ he has yet no personal experience of how the word in question actually is pronounced, then he simply does not know, and cannot teach the pronunciation of it. To go beyond such experience, and to say that the word ‘ought’ to be pronounced thus or thus, is to court disaster. These theoretical pronunciations, so far from being ‘refined’ or showing culture, are merely laughable. For if a speaker has not heard a word pronounced, what means can he possibly have for knowing what the sound of it ‘ought’ to be? There are, indeed, two ways by which he might arrive at a conclusion. The first, and the worst, and yet that usually employed by those who theorize about pronunciation, is the spelling; the other is the early history of the word in question, and of other words originally containing the same sound. To start with, let us say at once that neither of these tests will enable us to determine how the word ‘ought’ to have developed, since neither the schoolmaster nor the elocutionist can prescribe the path along which language shall change, any more than they can ‘bind the Unicorn, or EXPERIENCE ALONE TEACHES HOW TO PRONOUCE draw out Leviathan with an hook.’ Now as to how far either of the above methods can help us to arrive at what the pronunciation of a word is, which is the true object of our inquiry. The most unreliable of all guides to the pronunciation of an English word is its spelling, and nothing is more ludicrous than a theoretical pronunciation based solely upon it. On the other hand, a knowledge of the history of English sounds would certainly enable us to say, ‘The pronunciation may be so and so.’ It could not do more than suggest the possibilities; only a knowledge of the actual usage of the time could decide between the variously differentiated forms which our historical method would enable us to infer. For instance, a speaker (let us say a German philologist) who had never heard the word ‘good’ pronounced might know that O.E. gōd is capable of producing three types in Modern English (gūd, gud, gad), but he could not possibly say which is actually in use among ‘good speakers’ until he had gained the living experience.

As a matter of fact, any scholar so well versed in the history of English as to be able to reconstruct the possible forms of a word, would also know that, in Lord Chesterfield’s phrase, only the ‘usage of the best companies’ could decide between them.

In the case of words which are very rarely used, or which are revivals of obsolete forms, the tradition has naturally died out; there is no modern form, and the speaker who uses such words has his choice between the historical pronunciation (that which the word would probably have obtained if it had survived), or of a spelling pronunciation pure and simple. A curious example of a word which is really obsolete, because the institution which it denotes has passed away, is ‘chivalry.’ This word only survives in historical or romantic diction, and the old tradition has been lost. It is now very commonly pronounced (ʃivəlri), as if it were a word of recent importation from French, whereas it came into English through Norman-French; and there is no doubt that in that tongue, and in Middle English, it was pronounced (tʃivalrī), which would become (tʃivəlri) in Modern English. This pronunciation is indicated in Campbell’s lines:

‘Wave, Munich, all thy banners wave,

And charge with all thy chivalry,’

where the alliteration is obviously (tʃādž wið ɔ̄l ðaiivəlri).

The sport of falconry has practically died out in England, and both it, and the bird from which it takes its name, are known to most people only from books. The result is that the old pronunciation, without the l, has been lost, and the present pronunciation is due to the spelling. I have observed, however, that those few persons who have personal knowledge of the bird, and of the sport, invariably pronounce (fɔ̄kən, fɔ̄kənri), or at any rate the oldest generation do, instead of the now received (fɔ̄lkən). The general question of spelling-pronunciations which have become fixed and received will be discussed later on.

But if such artificial pronunciations are practically inevitable in the case of rare and obsolete words, they are inadmissible and ridiculous for words which are in common use, an which the speakers must have heard hundreds of times.

The chief cause of these absurdities occurring among educated speakers is a mistaken striving after refinement. VULGARITY OF SHAM REFINEMENT Public speakers, especially those whose traditions are purely academic rather than of a wider social world, are not infrequently guilty of extraordinary lapses from decorum and propriety in the matter of pronunciation.

It may seem incredible that men of learning, who convey the general impression that they expect to be taken seriously, should corrupt the English tongue to the extent of pronouncing (poignənt, læmb, litəratjɔ̄ə, rɑitias, fɔ̄hɛd, grīnwitʃ, sɑuðʌ̄n), all of which pronunciations the present writer has heard in the course of the last few years, instead of the ‘proper pronunciation’—in the sense of Lord Chesterfield—(poinənt, læm, litərətʃə, rɑitʃəs, fɔrid, grinidž, saðən). The speakers who perpetrated these forms pour rire must have known quite well what the ordinary pronunciation was; they must have been aware that their forms were deliberately falsified on the spur of the moment, from some vague idea of importing greater dignity (as they supposed) to their discourse. In these cases the speakers must have been anxious to deserve the praise, often ignorantly bestowed by the injudicious, that they ‘pronounced every letter distinctly.’ On the same principle, apparently, an eminent actor delights provincial audiences with the fervid expression of his (lov) ‘love.’

If we consider that we write many ‘letters’ in English spelling which represent no sound that has been heard in English speech for 500 years, or sometimes longer, it is easy to see that the practice, if consistently carried out, would result in an altogether unintelligible jargon, one which would, in most cases, resemble nothing that had ever existed in English, during the whole course of its history. It is a great fallacy to imagine that ‘Good English’ is to be obtained by distorting natural and usual pronunciation to suit some arbitrary standard of ‘refinement’ set up by an individual. Besides the monstrosities cited above, this effort at ‘refinement’ not infrequently leads to the production of strange and, in their context, quite un-English sounds, such as (ɛi, ē) instead of (ai) in ‘light,’ ‘rhyme,’ ‘prime,’ ‘desire,’ and so on, which has not even the specious justification of ‘giving every letter its full sound.’

The first pitfall to avoid, then, is a bogus ‘refinement’ of utterance.

The next error, closely allied to it, but often springing from a different motive, is over-carefulness. It may be laid down as a general principle that just as ‘refined’ speech such as we have been considering is always absurd, so ‘careful’ speech is always vulgar. The best English never conveys the impression of carefully-studied utterance on the part of the speaker; there is never any suspicion of mincing, as if to avoid some irretrievable vulgarism. This kind of pedantic and unreal pronunciation has nothing to be said in its favour. It may proceed from any one of the following causes: (1) Ignorance of the habitual pronunciation of good speakers. (2) A foolish desire to improve upon the received pronunciation, either by giving greater fulness, or, perhaps, even by introducing some sound which has either long disappeared, or has never existed at all; this motive is that wish for ‘refinement’ or ‘correctness’ already discussed. (3) In addressing a large audience public speakers feel a need for great precision, distinctness, and volume. To attain these ends they are sometimes unfortunately led into an exaggerated modification of their THE ‘LETTER’ WHICH KILLETH pronunciation, beyond the limits of the natural. We have already noted that there is a necessary and legitimate adaptation of speech under these circumstances, but a good speaker does not deviate so far from his natural modes of utterance as to produce something strange and manifestly artificial. It is surely absurd to maintain that the English of the present day is unfitted, in its natural form, for public oratory, and that it needs to be distorted for this purpose into something altogether different. (4) Many speakers have a curious sentimentality with regard to English. They are so solicitous of its purity and integrity, that practically no existing form of natural Spoken English comes up to their ideal of what the language ought to be. The ideal of this school is based entirely upon the present-day spelling. They may be quite ignorant of how that spelling came about, they may know nothing of the history of English pronunciation, but they show a remarkable tenderness for the letters, which they have come to think really are the word. This point of view is responsible for more eccentricities and affectations in pronunciation than any of the others, excepting, perhaps, that which aims at a personal distinction of utterance, as a kind of protest against the prevailing vulgarity. Both the speaker who wishes to speak better than anyone else, and the sentimentalist who lovingly clings to the ‘letters,’ are open to the grave reproach that they generally carry their vagaries into the colloquial speech of everyday life; and that while they are often fully conversant with polite usage, they yet deliberately set it at nought.

Assuming that a speaker had a thorough knowledge of the history of English pronunciation, it would, of course, be possible for him to select for his own usage the sound system in vogue in any century that he preferred. In this case he would at least be employing forms that had once had a real existence. Probably few would commend such a practice in speech, any more than they would welcome the return on the part of isolated individuals to the wigs of Charles the Second’s day, or the ruffs of the age of the first James. But the sentimental speaker of English is not as a rule familiar with any earlier phase of his language, but simply concocts a fancy dialect on the most unreliable of all bases—that of spelling, a guide which, as we have seen, is certain to lead the theorist into endless error.

The only safe course as regards pronunciation is frankly to recognise the fact that language changes, that standards of excellence shift, that the individual cannot delay the process, and that he is consulted as to which direction development will take.

The only good reason for deviating from the received standard of English speech is ignorance of it. The best substitute for such a form of English is a genuine provincial dialect, or an honest ‘vulgarism.’ For lack of knowledge may be informed, and, if necessary, a new dialect can be acquired.

The Teaching of Polite Pronunciation.

If it is desired to instruct those who do not possess it, in polite English pronunciation, there are three Perfect Points which demand attention, if success is to be attained. They are: The attitude of the teacher towards the actual dialect of the pupil; the setting up of true standards of TEACHING A NEW PRONUNCIATION speech; the method of imparting the new pronunciation. It is not too harsh a criticism on most of those who undertake this task, whether it be in schools, in training colleges, or among private pupils, in this country, to say that in the great majority of cases, the three points just mentioned do not meet with satisfactory or adequate treatment at their hands.

The instruction is given either by a regular elocutionist, or by any ordinary master or mistress, just as occasion serves. In the former case, the instruction, so far as it goes, is more or less systematic; in the latter it is purely haphazard, and takes the form of the occasional correction of isolated ‘mistakes’ as they occur in reading. The professed teacher of elocution, it is true, is primarily concerned with showing how poetry or prose should be read, in such a way as to ‘interpret the author’s meaning’; incidentally he also ‘corrects’ pronunciation. We may take the three points in order, and endeavour to state fairly the necessary shortcomings both of professional elocutionist and ordinary master or mistress.

The Attitude of the Teacher towards the Dialect of the Pupil.

The possession of a certain dialect as a native form of speech implies, as we know, the possession of a certain speech basis. The nature of this determines the natural tendencies and habits of pronunciation. If it is proposed to acquire a new and different pronunciation, a new speech basis must first be gradually formed. The first step in this process is for the speaker to know thoroughly, and understand, the facts of his own speech habits. Thence he can proceed to learn different habits.

Now, what is the practice of the inexperienced and untrained teacher of pronunciation? He brushes aside, as of no interest, no value, and as having no justification, the speech habits of a lifetime; he throws contempt or ridicule upon the pupil’s accent. His one idea is to ignore and forget the natural pronunciation of those whose speech he is to ‘improve.’ He asserts that it is ‘wrong,’ but he gives no reason for the statement; he abuses and disparages that which the pupil has learnt, from his mother, perhaps, and which he has heard and used himself so long as he can remember. He is quite ignorant of the ways of that ever-varying mystery, human speech; yet he takes upon himself to abuse and condemn a form of it which may have had a historical existence and development as ‘regular’ as Standard English itself, and which is, perhaps, a far purer dialect. He could not inform his class why his own speech ought to serve as a model, nor why it differs from theirs, nor, indeed, with any degree of accuracy, how it differs from theirs; yet he presumes to reiterate his own pronunciation of this or that word, and to assert that it is ‘Right.’ During the whole course of his instruction he never explains the meaning of the terms ‘Right’ and ‘Wrong,’ which he uses so often, beyond, perhaps, conveying the idea that the ‘wrong’ pronunciations of the students are bad attempts on their part to pronounce as he does himself.

Now, as most people with self-respect are keenly sensitive on the question of their language, such a method as that described (as it is believed without exaggeration), merely wounds without enlightening.

The Standards which are set up.

It is almost inevitable that a professional elocutionist, GOOD COLLOQUIAL ENGLISH THE BEST MODEL from his training, should seek his models of pronunciation and delivery, not in the best colloquial forms of English, but in the artificial declamatory utterance usual on the stage, or in high-flown public oratory. The standards, therefore, which he submits for the imitation of his pupils, and which he himself strives to illustrate in private converse, no less than in public recitation, are generally apt to be artificial to the last degree. There is a danger that, considered as types of public speaking, these standards will be archaic and pedantic; while as forms of colloquial speech they will be as far removed from the familiar pronunciation of good society as any dialect or out-and-out vulgarism could be. In this form of English we generally find all the distressing symptoms discussed above—over-carefulness, bogus refinement, impossible pronunciations, based, not on the fact of what is, but on a theory of what ‘ought’ to be. Undesirable as this kind of pronunciation is, even in public speaking, it is intolerable in private conversation; and he who practises it can hardly hope to escape the reproach of being a coxcomb and a pedant; he will certainly not pass for a well-spoken, well-bred person. We may grant that a competent teacher of elocution as such, even one who teaches on the above lines, has the power of imparting an intelligible and an expressive, if, perhaps, rather too ‘theatrical’ a delivery; but we can but feel that his method, even if considered as a training in public speaking only, is an inversion of the natural process. Before a man can speak well in public, he must first learn to speak well in private. The latter mode of speech must, above all things, be natural, and must not be based primarily upon models derived from public oratory, neither in pronunciation, nor in choice of diction. Good colloquial English, in a word, is not a modification of the English of the platform. On the other hand, it might with greater propriety be held that the best public speaking is a modified and adapted form of the best colloquial speech—of that which follows ‘the usage of the best companies.’ The teacher of elocution, by training and tradition, belongs to that sentimental order of persons, already referred to, who are jealous guardians of what they conceive to be the purity of English pronunciation, and strenuous opponents of new-fangled looseness and easy carelessness in utterance. He bewails the corrupt state into which the English language has fallen; he regards every pronunciation which differs from his own highly-wrought system as wrong and vulgar. So far from attempting to follow the best usage of his age in pronunciation, he denounces all natural pronunciation as slovenly, and wishes rather to lead contemporary speech into other paths, and to insist upon a pronunciation partly of his own making, partly delivered to him by tradition from those who taught him his craft. It will, perhaps, be apparent, from what has been already said concerning artificial pronunciations, that those who attempt to preserve an old pronunciation, rather than adopt that in common use, are in reality, too often the worst innovators, since they ‘restore,’ from insufficient knowledge, a pronunciation which has never existed, and which is entirely new. It is difficult to understand why it should be held that a new and natural development in language is a NEW TENDENCIES IN THE RISING GENERATION matter for regret. Modern English has slowly reached its present form by slow development, and has passed through numerous phases on its way thither from parent Aryan. By a series of minute but unceasing changes which have gone on during a period which a moderate estimate counts at 10,000 years, that far-off mother-tongue has passed here into Greek, there into Russian, there again into English, and into innumerable other forms of speech. Change may be slower in Modern English to-day than it was thousands of years ago in Central Europe, but none the less is the drama of transformation being enacted here as there. If it were not so, if it had not always been so, there could be no comparative philology, no possibility of ‘wrong’ speech, or ‘faulty’ delivery, and, consequently, no Art of Elocution; for Aryan speech would be undifferentiated, all individuals would speak alike—‘all the earth would be of one speech and one language.’

Whether this would have been an advantage or not we need not consider, for the fact is that language is always changing, and always will change. This being the case, the only reasonable attitude is that which observes and notes the changes as they occur, and accepts them with a good grace. Those who teach a younger generation must be prepared to find tendencies in the speech of their pupils which are absent from, or less fully developed in, their own. Careful observation over a wide field is necessary to enable us to distinguish these new tendencies, which are natural, and which are foreshadowings of future development, from other deviations from what we take to be Standard English, which are dialectal or personal peculiarities.

Methods of Teaching a New Pronunciation

We have already insisted so frequently, in the earlier chapters of this book, upon the importance of phonetics in the practical and historical study of language that it is unnecessary to return at any length to the question. It is enough to say that to learn a new pronunciation of the native language involves the same kind of difficulties as to learn any other new pronunciation. In approaching this practical side of linguistic study, mere imitation is inadequate and unsatisfactory, and systematic phonetic method is necessary. Since the proper pronunciation of a language includes two problems, the mastery of the right sounds, and the use of them in the right words, it will be found desirable, not only to make a phonetic analysis of the sounds of Standard English, which should be compared with that first made of the learner’s own sounds, but also to use texts in phonetic transcription which show the distribution of the sounds. The use of a simple phonetic alphabet should be practised, and the student should make transcripts of prose and verse in his own native pronunciation, and also take down his teacher’s pronunciation from dictation. It is, perhaps, necessary to warn those who ave not experience in this kind of work that the passages must be written down according to the natural pronunciation of the words in breath-groups, and not as consisting of isolated words. Thus, if Shenstone’s lines were dictated—

‘So sweetly she bade me adieu,

I thought that she bade me return,’

they should be read and taken down thus:

(‘Sou swītli ʃi bæd mi ədjū,

ai þɔ̄t ðət ʃi bæd mi ritʌ̄n’),

and not

(‘Sou swītli ʃī bæd mī ədjū,

ai þɔ̄t ðæt ʃī bæd mī ritʌ̄n’).

In this way the student learns, not only a natural instead of a pedantic and forced pronunciation of the sentence, but he also realizes how the sounds of words vary according to the degree of stress and the character of neighbouring sounds in any given context.

It should be remembered that very important elements in Polite English are proper stress, intonation, rate of utterance, and the accomplished use of the voice. Mr. Sweet in his New English Grammar has shown what vital elements stress and intonation are in English syntax. What is known as ‘over-emphasis’ is a vulgarism which must at all costs be eliminated. It consists in placing certain parts of the sentence in too strong a relief, by a disproportionate contrast between strong and weak stress, and also in allowing strong stress to recur too frequently in the breath-group. The result is a noisy clatter which suggests a series of jerks, instead of a quiet, even flow of speech, with occasional salient syllables strongly stressed, as good sense, good syntax, and good taste demand.

Intonation is the most difficult element in pronunciation to describe or to acquire. Vulgar speakers often affect the frequent use of compound tones to express persuasiveness, self-confidence, or good-natured cunning and sagacity. Good speakers avoid this means for the expression of these emotions, or use it very sparingly. The exaggerated use of the compound tones suggests impertinent familiarity. The Scotch peculiarity of finishing a sentence with a rising tone suggests querulousness, or cavilling, to English ears. One of the most characteristic features in a dialect is the precise degree of rise or fall, which it would demand to express with exactness a musical notation. Foreigners often produce a very curious effect by raising or lowering the pitch too much or too little as the case may be.

As regards the management of the speaking voice, nothing can make a poor voice into a good one; but an element in the best manner of speech is undoubtedly good resonance. In men a full chest note is usual among the best speakers, and a throttled, choky, wheezy utterance is not impressive. It is not given to everyone to possess a fine voice, but training and practice can give control and resonance even to a voice which is naturally weak and thin. Among certain classes of academic speakers a peculiar shrill, squeaky falsetto is in vogue, which we must pity as a misfortune in those who are naturally so afflicted, but which some will consider an absurd affectation in those who adopt it, being able to speak otherwise. This is probably another instance of that sham refinement too often deliberately acquired by the misguided. Among women shrill falsetto is rarely heard, except from those who have no pretentions to culture or manners. It is strange that some men, who represent the most fastidious and precious class in the world, should apparently have come to regard a squeaky voice as the sign of an enlightened mind and an exquisite taste. This manner of speech conveys the impression of querulous and impotent weakness, a quality in itself devoid of dignity and charm.

The Influence of Spelling on English Pronunciation.

The number of words in English, of which the ‘spelling INNOVATIONS IN PRONUNCIATION DUE TO SPELLING pronunciation’ has become current, in place of the traditional sound, is relatively small. An imposing list of these is given by Professor Koeppel, in his interesting little book, Spelling Pronunciations: Bemerkungen über den Einfluss des Schriftbildes auf den Laut im Englischen; Strassburg, 1901. (Quellen und Forschungen, Bd. lxxxix.) The principles which underlie this curious phenomenon are, in most cases, either the loss of the tradition of pronunciation of an obsolete word, which has been revived from literary sources as a semi-colloquial word; or, in the case of common, genuine colloquial words, the victory of a pedantic effort at refinement and correctness. In the case of proper names, the cause is often sheer ignorance of the traditional pronunciation, on the part of those who are strangers to a person or a place. With the arrival of the Railway in remote districts, porters, from London perhaps, din into the ears of travellers the name of the station, which they know chiefly from printed sources. The rising generation of natives very soon adopt the new pronunciation, and the mere tourist does so the more readily that he himself has no knowledge of the local, and therefore true, pronunciation. A few examples must suffice, as Professor Koeppel has dealt so copiously with the subject.

The name of St. Alphege is a good example of a literary revival, which, however, is not treated in his book. This saint’s day, as is recorded in the Prayer-Book Calendar, is April 19. A certain number of churches in England are dedicated to him, and he is (I believe) universally known at the present day as (sənt ælfɛdž). The O.E. form of the name is Ælfhe̅a̅h, which in Mod. English could only normally become either (ɛlvi) or (ælvi). The present actual pronunciation is apparently from a M.E. spelling Alpheȝ̇ȝ̇e (alfɛjɛ), which later on, when the memory of the stout old Archbishop had faded from men’s minds, and his name from their lips was spelt Alphegge or Alphege, and pronounced (ɑlfɛdž).

The pronunciation of ‘forward’ as (fɔ̄wəd) instead of the normal (fɔrəd) can only be the result of the same tendency which still makes some people say (fɔhɛd) instead of (fɔrid) or (fɔrɛd). But while the latter is still the sign either of a prig, or of one who is unacquainted with the speech of ‘the best companies,’ the former is the accepted and ‘correct’ form, except in the Navy, (fɔrəd) survives, of course, in provincial dialects, and in very colloquial speech among all classes.

The Fifeshire place-name Kilconquahar, which the present writer has heard old Fife people call (kɛnjahər) or (kɛnjūhər), is now apparently always called (kilkɔŋkər). The present writer can also remember the old-fashioned pronunciation of the Sussex villages Ardingly and Helingly as (ādiŋlɑi), or among the lower orders themselves (ærdiŋlɑi), and (hīliŋlɑi). These have now given place to (ādiŋli) and (hīliŋli). Sussex people still talk of (wɔdəst, midəst) for Wadhurst, Midhurst, and this is the pronunciation of the local gentry; but (wɔcdhʌ̄st, midhʌ̄st) are fast coming in through porters and trippers.

(sɑirinsɛstə), Cirencester, is more common now than either (sisitə) or (sisistə) even, or perhaps especially, among those who know the place quite well.

The village in which these words are written is locally THE CORRUPTION OF PROPOER NAMES known as (ɔ̄lskət) or (ælskət); but the inhabitant of this village, when he takes his ticket at Oxford Station, less than twenty miles away, is usually corrected by the booking-clerk, who insists on (ælvɛskɔt).

Lord Derby’s Lancashire seat Knowsley is almost universally called (nouzli), yet this pronunciation cannot conceivably have developed from M.E. Knouesli, or Knou(l)wesli, O.E. Kenulfes le̅a̅h. The true descendant of the old forms is heard in the now ‘vulgar’ (nɑuzli), which, I am told, still persists among the aged in the district.

In fact, English Place-names are now so generally corrupted in their pronunciation through the influence of spelling, that in many cases it is impossible to understand the connection between the old forms and the current pronunciation. It becomes, therefore, of the utmost importance to ascertain the true pronunciation among old people in the district itself, and to pay but small attention, until this is done, either to the spelling, or to the conventional pronunciation, if we wish to trace the history of the name. In the case of other English words, whose modern forms do not square with the older forms, as regards normal sound change, the possibility of a corrupt modern pronunciation, based upon the spelling, must be borne in mind. We should rather assume this, than an ‘exception’ to the known tendencies of change in the language.

We occasionally hear peculiarly flagrant breaches of polite usage, such as (iz nɔt it) for (iznt it) or (æm not ai), for the now rather old-fashioned, but still commendable, int ɑi) or the more usual and familiar (ānt ɑi), or, in Ireland (æmnt ɑi). These forms, which can only be based upon an uneasy and nervous stumbling after ‘correctness,’ are perfectly indefensible, for no one ever uttered them naturally and spontaneously. They are struck out by the individual, in a painful gasp of false refinement. There is little chance of such abortive creations getting a secure foothold in traditional English, unless linguistic education becomes altogether divorced from life, and until the native language is taught as though it were a dead language, with which the schoolmaster had but an imperfect acquaintance.


This imperfect treatment of a great subject must now draw to a close. The mere thought of human speech, passed on from lip to lip through unnumbered ages, changing along a definite path among each race as it flashes through them, unconsciously shaped to the needs of every mind, which it mirrors, and yet, in spite of all, preserving an identity which the ear of science can recognise, is one which must kindle a strange sense of wonder and reverence. The most commonplace form of language which we can think of has an ancestry more ancient than any custom or myth which survives. The humblest form of English, whether spoken in a remote Devonshire hamlet or in a Northern pit village, is an echo of a tongue that once sounded in far-distant countries, among alien and savage men, and in ages possibly, when the present configuration of the globe was not yet determined.

Language, so familiar, and yet so mysterious, lies all about us. The human mind and the human vocal organs, the one more complex, the others defter, than in the remote CONCLUSION past, but still essentially the same now as then, are an ever-present field for the observation of the student. The root of all science may lie in an awakened and alert curiosity concerning the obvious and the commonplace.

This little book could find no more fitting conclusion than the words of Ælfric, in the Preface of his Lives of the Saints:

‘No secge we nan þing niwes on þissere gesetnysse,

forþan þe hit stod gefyrn awriten

. . . þeah þe þa læwedan men þæt nyston.’

‘We say nothing new in this work, for it all stood written long ago, albeit laymen did not know it.’


SUBJECT INDEX

Ablaut, nature of, 163;

  in Aryan, 182;

  name due to Grimm, 183;

  accent and, 184, 185;

  grades, 185, 186, 187;

  quantitative, 184;

  qualitative, 184, 188;

  diphthongal combinations in, 189;

  examples of, 190-194.

Accent, Aryan, 184;

  Parent Germanic, 199.

Alphabet, International, 50.

Analogy, ‘exceptions’ due to, 115, 213;

  process of, 129;

  memory and, 129;

  ‘false,’ 132;

  mistakes due to, 132, 133;

  results in new formations, 134, 135;

  prevention of differentiation by, 136;

  normal sound change and, 137;

  continual process of, 138-140.

Anglo-French, 288, 289.

Anglo-Frisian Unity, views of Siebs and Bremer, 195;

  Morsbach and Wyld, 196.

Archaisms, revival of, 127.

Arnold, Matthew, appreciation of Dryden, 344.

Aryan, Mother-tongue, 8, 9, 170, 171;

  reconstructed forms, value of, 144;

  relative homogeneity of, 103;

  wealthy vowel system of, 161;

  divisions of, 169, 373;

  race, 172, 173;

  its cradle, 171, 172;

  relative primitiveness of chief divisions, 173, 174;

  mutual relations of these, 175-181;

  consonants, 181;

  vowels, 182;

  ablaut, 182-194;

  accent, 184;

  Modern English and, 373.

Association groups, 130-131;

  levelling of exceptions due to, 133;

  isolation from, 135, 136.

Avesta, the, dialect of, 169.

 

Barbour’s ‘Bruce,’ rhymes in, 262.

Björkman, remarks on Scandinavian loan-words in O.E., 249;

  on close resemblance between English and Norse, 282.

Bopp, Franz, 8;

  views on sound change, 82.

Brugmann asserts inadmissibility of ‘exceptions,’ 114;

  principles of method used in reconstruction, stated by, 163;

  works of, 166;

  views of Aryan affinities, 179, 180;

  on reduced vowels, 186, 187;

  principles of philological method formulated by, 215.

Bülbring on pronunciation of O.E. cȝ, 225.

 

Caxton, Literary English and, 294;

  London dialect and, 295, 296, 297.

Chaucer, persistence of Norman-French accent in, 123;

  Literary dialect and, 251;

  rhymes of, 259;

  O.E. œ̆, œġ in, 265;

  French influence on language of, 289;

  London dialect and, 296, 297;

  Canterbury Tales, expression in, 345.

Chesterfield, Lord, his definition of correct speech, 361, 362;

  condemns trite phrases, 351.

Cognates, examples of, 142;

  tests of identity of origin, 142.

Comparison, reconstruction based on, 142, 150;

  words suitable for, 143;

  conditions necessary for, 142;

  limitations within one language, 145, 147;

  importance of early forms for, 145, 146, 147;

  light thrown by widening range of, 147-149, 155-163;

  limitations, within one speech-family of, 151-155.

Consonants, classification of, 32-35;

  natural series of, 35, 36;

  long and double, 48.

Conversation, Language of, independent life of, 340, 341;

  adaptation to environment, 347, 348;

  limits of adaptation, 348, 349.

‘Correctness’ in language, standard of, 353;

  fluctuation of standard of, 359;

  Lord Chesterfield’s definition of, 361, 362.

Corruptions, 12;

  common use of the term, 19.

 

Darmsteter, views on sound change, 84.

‘Dialect’ and ‘language’ compared, 91.

Dialects, mixture of, 22;

  tests of relative superiority of, 22, 23;

  importance of study of, 25, 26, 205;

  rise of, 95, 96;

  class, 99;

  artificial, literary, 212;

  decay of English, 104;

  scientific view of equality among, 353, 354;

  absence of standard in, 357;

  subordinate position of, 357, 358;

  linguistic development in, 358;

  standard, artificiality in, 358, 359.

Dryden, French influence in, 289;

  appreciation by Matthew Arnold, 344;

  prose of, 344, 345.

 

Ellis interprets authorities on pronunciation, 67, 68, 301, 309.

English, development of vocabulary of, 209;

  modified inflexional system of, 208;

  Norman words in, 124;

  Scandinavian words in, 124;

  Indian words in, 124;

  lifeless forms of, 349-353.

English, Correct, practical advantages of its study and use, 352, 353.

English dialects, decay of, 104.

English, Good, reality of existence of, 342.

English, History of, what it involves, 205;

  methods of study, 205, 206.

English, Literary, ‘sounds’ of, inaccurate use of term, 341, 342;

  sources of, 251, 342, 343;

  rise of, 294-297;

  Chaucer and, 251;

  Wycliff and, 251;

  Gower and, 251;

  Caxton and, 294;

  Standard English and, 251, 295, 340-346.

English, Middle, apparently exceptional spellings in, 210, 211;

  relation to Modern English, 250;

  authorities on, 252, 253;

  chronological divisions, 253;

  dialects, 253, 254;

  texts, 254, 255;

  orthography, 255-259;

  pronunciation, how established, 259, 260;

  sound changes in, 260-265;

  treatment of O.E. diphthongs, 265, 266;

  rise of new diphthongs in, 266, 267;

  vowel-lengthening, 268, 269;

  vowel-shortening, 270-273;

  doublets in, 273;

  treatment of O.E. consonants, 273-280;

  O.E. ċ, and ċġ, difficulties concerning, in, 275-277;

  summary of dialectal differences in, 280;

  French element in, 287-289;

  inflexions, 289-293;

  Scandinavian element, 281-284;

  tests of Scandinavian origin, 285-287.

English, Modern, development of M.E. vowels in, 309-330;

  ā̆, statements of authorities concerning, 309-316;

  ā̆, summary of development of, 316, 317;

  e, 317, 318;

  ē ‘tense,’ 319, 320;

  ī, oi, 321-323;

  ē ‘slack,’ 320, 321;

  ō ‘tense,’ 323-327;

  ō tense, Scotch pronunciation of, 324;

  ō ‘slack,’ development of, 324, 325;

  ŏ, 325;

  ŭ, 325-327;

  ŭ in Scotch dialects, 328;

  ū, 328;

  ȳ, 329, 330;

  treatment of M.E. diphthongs, 330-336;

  ɑi, ei, development of, 330-332;

  ɑu, 333-336;

  ou, 336;

  consonants, development of, in, 336-338;

  slow development of, 373;

  Aryan and, 373.

English, Old, problems presented by MSS., 210;

  significance of ‘exceptional’ spellings, 210;

  stages of development, 216;

  dialects, 216-217;

  sources of knowledge of, 217;

  texts, 217-220;

  monographs on, 220-222;

  pronunciation, 222, 223;

  values of vowel symbols, 223-224;

  pronunciation of consonants, 224-225;

  symbols, 224, 225;

  ċ, g, ġ, ċġ in, 225;

  authorities on pronunciation of, 225-226;

  books for beginners on, 226;

  W. Germanic vowel changes affecting, 227-231;

  an, on in, 229;

  Fracture or ‘Brechung,’ 229-231;

  nasals, loss of, 232, 233;

  i-mutation, 233, 234;

  lengthening of vowels, 235;

  dialectal divergences, 235-238;

  Celtic loan-words, 238-239;

  Latin loan-words, 239-248;

  Scandinavian loan-words, 248-249;

  native words adapted to Christian uses, 247, 248.

English Place-Names, 378, 379.

English, Polite, 342;

  rate of utterance in, 375.

English, Spoken, historical study and, 205, 339;

  first steps in study of, 339, 340;

  source of, 341;

  use of term, 341;

  importance of study of, 206, 353;

  standards of Good or Bad, 353-357.

English of the Stage, 355, 356.

English, Standard, existence of, 23;

  historical position of, 24;

  varying standard of, 24, 25, 318, 359;

  uniformity in, 22-25, 101, 102, 354;

  spread of, 104, 105;

  source of, 251, 295, 342, 343;

  provincial speech and, 297, 298;

  changes in, 299;

  Literary English and, 251, 295, 340-346;

  existence and growth of, 342, 356;

  nature of, 342;

  artificiality of, 343, 357-364;

  adaptability of, 346-349;

  checks upon differentiation in, 354;

  pronunciation, chief criterion of, 355;

  where heard, 355;

  possible divergences in, 355, 359, 360;

  importance of, for teacher, etc., 356;

  ‘absolute’ superiority of, 24, 357;

  influence of fashion on, 327, 359.

Environment, Influence of, 63;

  normally unperceived, 63;

  gradually lessens, 64.

Esperanto, 105;

  its probable future, 105-109.

Exceptions, explanations of apparent, 114-115, 212-214, 379.

 

Foreign words, translations of, 122;

  conditions for incorporation of, 122, 123.

 

Germanic, 8, 168, 196;

  divisions of, 195;

  authorities, 196;

  sources of knowledge of, 197;

  characteristics of, 197;

  consonant shifting, 198-201;

  ‘free’ accent, 199;

  treatment of Aryan vowels, 202-203;

  West, characteristics, 203.

Glides, 44;

  p, t, k, in English and French, 44.

Gower, and the literary dialect, 251;

  distinguishes between tense and slack ē, 257.

Grammar, comparative and historical, 9.

Grassmann’s Law, 174.

Greek, faithfully preserves primitive vowel system, 160, 174;

  Grassmann’s Law, 174.

Grimm’s Law, 197, 198.

 

Hirt, views on sound change, 85, 87, 179;

  on reduced vowels, 186, 187, 191.

Historical linguistic study, 1-3;

  aim, 6;

  methods of, 4-10, 211-215;

  necessary equipment for, 10-11;

  proper basis of, 61, 206, 339.

 

Imitation, limitations of, 56, 374;

  dangers of faulty, 58, 59;

  native tongue learnt by, 54;

  sound change and faulty theories concerning, 84;

  changes due to faulty, 125.

i-mutation, 10, 150, 233, 234.

Intonation, 47;

  in Polite English, 375, 376.

 

-jan suffix in Gothic and Old Saxon, 148.

 

Kluge on pronunciation of O.E. cȝ, 225, 226;

  Scandinavian words in O.E., 249.

 

Language, continual change in, 14, 373.

Language, Life of, psychological aspect, 11, 13;

  physiological, 11, 13.

Language, Literary, danger of exclusive study of, 11, 13;

  position with regard to spoken language, 12, 340, 341;

  comparatively archaic, 344;

  sources of, 341.

Language, Spoken, limitations, 5;

  changes in, 14;

  writing and, 5, 62;

  unconscious process of, 61, 62, 63;

  importance of study of, 10, 11, 13, 206;

  advantage of training in facts of, 340;

  independent life of, 340, 341;

  influence of literature on, 341;

  adaptability of, 346.

Language, Standard, two aspects of history of, 361.

Language transmission, changes involved in, 65.

Language, Written, use of term, 341.

Latin, corruptions in, 12;

  the primitive vowels and diphthongs in, 174;

  the primitive consonants in, 174.

Leskien asserts inadmissibility of ‘exceptions,’ 114, 117;

  position of, in linguistic science, 166;

  modifies ‘Übergangstheorie,’ 177-179.

Linguistic contact, through literature, 125, 126;

  introduction of foreign elements, 122-124.

Loan-words, development indicated by, 121, 122;

  points of interest concerning, 209;

  popular fallacies concerning, 209;

  test of source of, 210;

  importance of form, 245;

  Scandinavian, 248, 249, 281-285;

  Latin, 239-248;

  Celtic, 238-239;

  tests for Scandinavian origin of, 285-287.

London Dialect, Standard English and, 294-298, 342, 343.

 

Max Müller, original home of the Aryans, views on, 171.

Memory Pictures, 57-59;

  gradual alteration of, 72;

  subconscious, 70, 71.

Mistake,’ significance of term, 19, 20.

 

Napier, Professor, his discovery of Orm’s new symbol, 258.

 

Obsolete Forms, possible pronunciations of, 363, 364.

Orm, value of his orthography, 256;

  establishes M.E. quantities, 260, 268, 269, 270, 271, 272;

  his new symbol for back-stop (g), 258.

Osthoff defines ‘correctness’ in language, 21;

  views on sound change, 83;

  asserts inadmissibility of ‘exceptions,’ 114;

  position in science of language, 166.

 

Passy, views on sound change, 84, 90.

Paston Letters, Oxford dialect and, 296.

Paul, remarks on relation of individual speaker to community, 103;

  asserts inadmissibility of ‘exceptions,’ 114;

  position of, in science of language, 166;

 ‘Wellentheorie,’ views on, 177.

Philology, comparative, meaning of, 8;

  task of, 141;

  advance of science of, 142;

  method of, 143, 144.

Phonetic Analysis, pronunciation and, 374.

Phonetic Laws, meaning of term, 112;

  nature of, 117;

  exceptions to, inadmissible, 114.

Phonetic practice, 60;

  exercises, 35, 36, 38, 39, 40, 41.

Phonetic symbols, 50, 51;

  tables of, 52, 53;

  explanation of, 54;

  usefulness of, 374.

Phonetic training, ingenious objections to, 16, 17;

  importance of, 15, 374;

  what it involves, 17, 18, 27, 59, 60;

  why advantageous, 18, 19;

  proper basis for, 27, 60, 61, 339;

  historical linguistic study and, 339.

Phonological investigation, nature and importance of, 113.

Place-names, English, 378, 379.

Pogatscher, views on use of Latin by Britons, 242, 243.

Pronunciation, spelling and, 14, 15, 116, 212;

  sixteenth-century, authorities on, 302-304;

  seventeenth-century, authorities on, 304, 305;

  eighteenth-century, authorities on, 306;

  interpretation of authorities on, 307, 308;

  of fashion on, 327, 359;

  varieties within Standard Dialect of, 359, 360;

  varieties indicating difference of dialect, 361;

  English spelling and 363, 377-38;

  vulgarity of ‘overcarefulness’ in, 366, 367;

  difficulties involved in unfamiliar, 374.

Pronunciation, Correct, decided by experience, 362, 363.

Pronunciation ‘Good,’ criteria of, 361-368.

Pronunciation, Polite, teaching of, 368-376;

  present methods of teaching, criticism of, 369-371.

Pronunciations, Spelling, 377-379;

  absurdity of, 364-366;

  causes of, 365, 377, 378, 380.

Prose, natural language of good, 345.

Public Speaking, 346, 347, 348.

 

Quantity, 47, 48.

 

Reconstruction, possibility of, 142;

  test of accurate, 151;

  principles of, 163, 164;

  necessity of, 206;

  varying methods of, 207;

  Modern period, problem of, 300.

Reconstructed Forms, value of, 144.

‘Right’ and ‘Wrong’ in Language, definition of, 21, 129;

  analogy and, 132, 139;

  scientific and practical views of, 353;

  constant change in, 353;

  Standard Dialects and, 358;

  no ‘absolute’ standard of, 361;

  ignorant use of terms, 370.

Rig-Veda, hymns of, 169.

 

Salesbury, William, 301.

Sanscrit, a, an in Lithuanian and, 156, 157;

  sounds in Greek and Latin corresponding to a in, 156-159;

  palatalization in, 159, 160;

  vowel system less primitive than Greek, 160, 174;

  consonants relatively primitive in, 174.

Scherer, views on sound change, 82;

  position of, in science of language, 166.

Schleicher, views on sound change, 82;

  his ‘Stammbaum’ theory, 175, 178, 180.

Schmidt, Johann, original home of Aryans, views on, 171;

  attacks ‘Stammbaum’ theory, 176;

 ‘Wellentheorie,’ 176, 177, 178, 179.

Schrader accepts Schmidt’s ‘Wellentheorie,’ 177.

Scotch, sixteenth-century ŭ in dialects of, 328;

  O.E. ō tense in, 328.

Scots, distinguished history of, 208.

Seek, ‘beseech’ and, 145, 146, 147;

 ‘sought’ and, 147-150.

Shakespeare, reconstruction of his pronunciation, 207.

Sievers, use of term ‘bedingt,’ 81;

  asserts inadmissibility of ‘exceptions,’ 114;

  position of, in science of language, 166;

  on pronunciation of O.E. cȝ, 225;

  on -n- verbs in O.E., 284.

Skeat, on French element in English, 287, 288.

Sound change, fact of, 14, 15;

  evidence of, in written records, 67;

  in cognate forms, 68;

  inaccuracy of term, 69;

  process of, 70, 71, 72, 73;

  cause of, 73, 81;

  isolative, 73, 74;

  combinative, 75-77, 214;

  transitoriness of tendencies, 76-78, 373;

  theories in explanation of, 82-85;

  caused by foreign contact, 85-87;

  occupation as factor in, 88;

  inadequacy of theories to explain it, 89;

  spread of, 110, 111;

  unconscious nature of, 113;

  importance of study of, 113;

  laws of, 111, 112;

  analogy and, 137.

Sound changes, Old English, 226, 232, 233;

  West Germanic affecting Old English, 227-232;

  Middle English, 260-265;

  Modern English, 309, etc. (see English, Modern).

Sound Laws, meaning of term, 77;

  admit of no exceptions, 114, 117.

Speech of a Town, how far homogeneous, 99.

Speech basis, 70;

  factors involved in, 81;

  influence of race on, 86;

  influenced by physical type, 87;

  change in, 87;

  by occupation, 88;

  foreign sounds modified by native, 120, 121.

Speech communities, meaning of term, 92-93;

  possibilities and limitations of change in, 93, 94;

  relative homogeneity within, 94, 109;

  contact between, 119-121;

  modes of isolation of, 97, 98.

Speech, ‘correct,’ popular view of, 21;

  scientific conception of, 21, 129.

Speech family, Aryan, existence of, 8;

  Aryan, divisions of, 169;

  conception of, 166-168.

Speech habits, formation of, 58, 59.

Speech, Individual, various influences on, 100-102;

  divergence originates from, 103, 104.

Speech, Living, essentials of, 349.

Speech sounds, classification of, 28-31;

  processes involved in utterance of, 56, 57, 58.

Spelling, English, fixed, 15;

  pronunciation and, 14, 15, 116, 212;

  Middle English, 116, 255-259;

  English pronunciation and, 363, 377-380.

Spelling Pronunciations, absurdity of, 364, 365, 366;

  in English, 377-380;

  causes of, 365, 377, 378, 380.

‘Stammbaum’ theory, Schleicher and the, 175;

  Johann Schmidt, attack on, 176, 177;

  Leskien, views on, 177, 178.

Standard, constant shifting of, 353, 359.

Stereotyped Phrases, 350, 351;

  effect of use of, 351;

  Lord Chesterfield’s opinion of, 351.

Streitberg on lengthening of original short vowels, 186.

Stress, 45;

  degrees of, 46;

  distribution of, 46;

  importance of, 106;

  preservation of, 123;

  Ablaut and, 184;

  doublets due to, 215;

  in Polite English, 375.

Sweet, improves Organic Method, 28;

  use of terms ‘narrow’ and ‘wide,’ 39, 40;

  discovers ‘shifted’ vowels, 42;

  his phonetic symbols, 50, 51;

  remarks on ‘exceptional’ forms, 132;

  on pronunciation of O.E. cȝ, 225;

  remarks on -an and -on forms in Old English, 229;

  his divisions of Middle English, 253;

  on Scandinavian verbs with -n- suffix, 284;

  discusses problems of Modern English pronunciation, 309;

  on development of au, 333;

  spoken English, indicates method of study of, 339;

  on importance of stress and intonation in English, 375.

Syllable, limits of, 50;

  division, 48, 49.

 

Texts, O.E., 217-220;

  M.E., 254, 255.

Tunþus-tōþ-dent, etc., methods of comparison and reconstruction illustrated by, 151-163.

 

Verner’s Law, 198, 199, 200.

Voice, management of, in speech, 376.

Vowels, consonants and, 31;

  analysis of, 37;

  tongue activities for, 37;

  muscular activities for, 39;

  lip activity for, 40, 41;

  description of, 41, 42;

  positions, 42;

  difficulty of ‘low-front,’ 38;

  ‘shifted,’ 42;

  intermediate varieties of, 43.

Vulgarism,’ 19.

 

Wechsler, views on sound change, 85, 87.

‘Wellen’ or ‘Übergangstheorie,’ Johann Schmidt and the, 176, 177;

  Schrader, views on, 177;

  Paul, views on, 177;

  Leskien, modification of, 177-179.

Whitney, views on sound change, 82.

Wright, views on the use of Latin in Britain, 242, 243.

Wycliff, Literary dialect and, 251;

  Oxford type and, 296, 297.


WORD INDEX

n = note. > = derived from. dial. = dialectal, obs. = obsolete. Square brackets indicate phonetic spelling.

Sanskrit.

abhi-jnú, 193

aditas, 192

ajami, 157

ajñasam, 194

ajras, 157, 191

anti, 156

asti, 157

ašva, 157

avi-, 157

 

bandhus, 156, 162

bhrā́-tar, 201

 

ca, 157

catvâras, 160

 

dadarša, 157

dadāti, 192

dádhāmi, 192, 201

dadhmas, 192

damas, 156

dant-, 155, 161

dēváttas, 192

dhūmas, 68

ditiš, 192

 

gōštha, 193

 

hitás, 192

 

jambha, 156

janas, 156

jā́nu, 193

jñātás, 194

 

kákša, 160

kakúd, 160

katara, 157

 

madhu, 157

mati-, 162

 

nasā́, 191

 

pā̆d-, 142

panca, 160

pāni, 74

pári-jman, 191

parīnas, 194

pátati, 157

pati, 157, 198

pitár, 200

pr̥nāti, 194

pūrnás, 194

 

saptá, 199

šatám, 112, 162

sthitás, 193

stighnutē, 201

stri, 192

svašrū́, 200

 

tam, 156

 

ukşan, 204

— — — —

Greek.

ἀγρός, 157, 191

ἄγω, 157, 191

ἀ-κούω, 202

ἄκτωρ,191

ἀντὶ, 156

ἀ-σκηθής 199

 

βαίτη (Thracian), 201

 

γένος, 156

γέρανος, 201

γι-γνώ-σκω, 194

γνύξ, 193

γόνυ, 193

γομφίoς, 156

γόμφος, 156

γωνία, 193

 

δαμάω, 201

δέδορκε, 157

δίδομεν, 192

δίδωμι, 186, 192

δί-φρ-ος, 190

δόμος, 156

δοτός, 186

δώσω, 192

 

ἔδω, 203

 

ἕζομαι, 190, 203

ἑκατόν, 112, 162

ἑκυρᾱ́, 200

εἰμί, 157

ἕνος, 157

επί-βδ-αι, 190

ἑπτά, 199

ἔργον, 203

ἐστί, 157

ἔφυγον, 182

 

ἦχε, 191

 

θατός, 187

θετός, 186

θῡμός, 68

 

ἵ-σταμεν, 193

ιστᾱμι, 186

ἵστημι, 193

 

καρδία, 199

κυριακά, 240

 

λέγω, 182

ληδεῖν, 192

λόγος, 182

λύκος, 290

 

μέθυ, 157

μέσσος, 203

 

νῆ-μα, 202

 

ὀδμή, 191

ὀ-δόντ-, 155, 161

ὀδωδή, 191

οἴνη, 202

ὄις, 157, 202

ὄπ-ωπ-α, 191

ὄσσε, 191

οῦς, 202

ὄψομαι 191

 

πατέρα, 182, 291

πατηρ, 182, 190, 200

πέζα, 190

πείθω, 202

πενθερός, 156, 162, 163, 203

πέντε, 160

πέσσαρες, 160

πέτε-ται, 157

πλῆ-ρες, 194

ποδός, 190

πόσις, 157, 198

πότερος, 157

πούς, 142

πρόχνυ, 193

πρωί, 194

πῶς (Doric), 190

 

ῥητήρ,ῥήτωρ, 183, 184

 

στατός, 186, 193

στείχω, 201

στήσω, 193

σφάλλω, 198

 

τὲ, 157

τιθεμεν, 192

τίθημι, 186, 192, 201, 202

τόν, 156

 

φαμέν, 193

φᾱμί (Doric), 182

φέρω, 190

φεύγω, 182

φημί, 193

φορᾱ́, 190

φρᾱ́-τηρ, 190

φρᾱ́-τωρ, 190, 201, 202

φρά-τρ-α, 190

φώγω, 192

φωνή, 182

φώρ, 190

 

ὢψ, 191

— — — —

Latin.

actor, 191

ager, 157

ago, 157, 191

ambāges, 191

ante, 156

appodix, 190

auctor, 190

auris, 202

 

cacūmen, 160

Cæsar, 241

capistrum, 244

cāseus, 241

centum, 112, 162, 163

colonia, 244

coquere, 158

coquīna, 76, 244

cordis, 199

coxa, 160

cuculla, 244

cucurbita, 242

 

Dānuvius, 201

dāre, 182

datio, 192

datus, 182, 192

dēdi, 192

dent-, 155, 161, 163

domare, 201

domus, 156

dōnare, 192

dōnum, 182, 192

 

edo, 203

equus, 157

est, 157

exāmen, 191

 

facio, 192

fœnuculum, 244

fāma, 193

fāri, 193

fēci, 192

fēmella, 134

fero, 190

fīcus, 241

fīdo, 202

fors, 190

fortūna, 190

frāter, 202

fūmus, 68

fūr, 190

 

genus, 156

 

hominem, 291

hospitis, 198

hostis, 202

 

lassus, 192

 

marmor, 244

medius, 203

memini, 182

ment-, 162

mentum, 199

mercātum, 248

moneo, 182

moneta, 241

morātum, 244

mūtare (> moitare), 202

 

nāpus, 241

nāres, 191

nāsus, 191

nēre, 202

nīdus, 190, 203

nōsco, 194

nox, 157

 

oculus, 191

odor, 191

offendix, 162, 163, 203

oleum, 234

ovis, 157, 202

 

pater, 190

patria, 122

patris, 190

pedem, 190

pedes, 203

pēs, 142, 190, 290

petit, 157

piscis, 113

plēnus, 194

præpositus, 244

psalmus, 244

 

que, 157

quinque, 160

 

rego, 239

regula, 79

rēx, 239

ruta, 244

 

sāgire, 202

satus, 192

sedēre, 190, 203

sēdimus, 190

sēmen, 192

senex, 157

sēvi, 192

sodālis, 190

stāmen, 193

stāre, 193

statim, 193

status, 193

strāta via, 241

 

tabula, 242

tego, tēxi, 182

 

uncia, 245

ūnus > oinos, 202

 

veho, vēxi, 186

— — — —

Gallo-Roman.

Moguntiacum, 158

 

Vosegus, 158

— — — —

Old French.

femelle, 134

— — — —

French.

beau, 53

bête, 48

bon, 30

but, 41, 51

 

content, 54

 

dé, 53

dur, 41

 

enfant, 76

été, 39, 40

 

fin, 30

fini, 31

français, 35

 

génie, 123

 

jamais, 35

 

lune, 38

 

rendre, 35

 

si, 53

 

un, 30

 

vu, 41, 51

— — — —

Old Irish.

āg, 191

 

brocc, 239

 

cethir, 160

 

drui, 239

 

rī, rīg, 239

— — — —

Irish.

donn, 239

 

iasc, 113

— — — —

Welsh.

dwn, 239

Llandudno, 35

— — — —

Gothic.

ains, 202

akrs, 157, 191

andbundnan, 154

anþar, 152, 153

augō, 228

auhsa, 204

ausō, 202

awistr, 193, 202

 

bairan, 190

bandi, 154

bar, 190

batists, 150

batiza, 284

baur, 190

beidan, 202

bērum, 190

bindan, 154, 203

brōþar, 190, 201, 202

broþrahans, 190

bug-jan, 148

 

dags, 227

dauns, 68

dōmjan, 10

drōbjan, 148

 

fadar, 200

-faþs, 198

fōdjan, 148

fōtus, 142, 190

fruma, 194

fulljan, 148

fulls, 194

 

gabinda, 154

gadēþs, 192, 201, 202

gaf, 182

gaits, 228

gamōtjan, 148

gamunds, 162

gasinþa, 154

gasinþja, 152

gastim (dat.), 290

gasts, 202

gatamjan, 201

gēbum, 182

giban, 182

 

haims, 228

hairtō, 199

handus, 154, 183

hansa, 152

haubiþ, 228

hausjan, 202, 236

-hinþan, 154, 183

huggrjan, 148

hund, 112, 153, 162

hunsl, 247

hunþs, 154

 

juggs, 153

 

kaisar, 241

kann, 194

kaus, 182

kinnus, 76

kiusan, 182

kniu, 193

knussjan, 193

kuni, 77, 234

kunnaida, 194

kunþs, 153

kusum, 182

 

lats, 192

lētan, 192

 

maidjan, 202

mana-sēþs, 192

midjis, 203

munþs, 152, 153, 199

 

namnjan, 148

nēþla, 202

 

paida, 201

 

reiki, 239

reiks, 239

 

sandjan, 154

sat, 190

satjan, 148

sētum, 190

sibun, 199

sinþs, 152, 154, 232

skaþjan, 199

sōkjan, 147, 202

staþs, 193

steigan, 201

stōls, 193

 

tunþus, 151, 153, 161, 163

 

þāhta, 228, 231

þagkjan, 231

þana, 156

 

unkja, 245

 

warjan, 148

-windan, 154

-winnan, 154

wulfs, 290

— — — —

Old Norse.

bleikr, 286

 

fōtr, 142

 

geva, 279

 

heimsōcn, 283

hvītna, 284

 

lǣte, 261

lāt, 261

 

mjūkr, 287

 

skamt, 285

sōma, 282

sveinn, 283

 

tannr, 153

 

veikr, 286

— — — —

Old West Scandinavian.

blikna, 284

bustla, 284

 

dogg, 286

 

egg, 286

 

hoggua, 286

 

tryggr, 286

— — — —

Old Swedish.

batna, 284

— — — —

Swedish.

babbla, 284

 

dagg, 286

dangla (dial.), 284

 

en, 167

 

fem, 167

 

fyra, 167

 

höra, 167

hörde, 167

 

komma, 167

 

moder, 167

 

tre, 167

twå, 167

— — — —

Danish.

dag, 167

 

sang, 167

skygge, 286

synge, 167

sunget, 167

— — — —

Old English.

Abbod, 247

 

æcer, 191, 227

ǣg, 286

ælfhe̅a̅h, 378

ælmesse, 247

ǣr, 262

 

āld, 45, 236, 260, 323

ān-buend, 247

ān-setl, 247

ār, 269

ā-wæcnian, 284

 

bacan, 192

bæcere, 192

bær, 190, 213

bǣron, 190, 214

bānd, 273

barda, 281

beald, bāld, 45, 236

bēċ, 133

beġinnan, 278

be̅o̅, 319

beran, 190, 213, 259, 319

beter, 284

betst, 150

bīdan, 202

bindan, 154, 203

blāc, 286

blōd, 323

bōc, 192

boren, 190, 214

bræc, 213

brǣþan, brēþan, 6

brǣþ, brēþ, 6

breogo, 79

bre̅o̅st, 272

bringan, 231

brocc, 239

brōhte, 231, 274

brōþor, 134, 201, 202

bryċġ, 238, 258

brȳsan, 264

byċġan, 148

byrġean, 237

 

cæfester, 244

cāld, 75, 236

cāmb, 156, 235

cāsere, 241

ce̅a̅c, 257

ċeaf, cafu, 277

ċeald, 231, 236

ċe̅a̅pmenn, 270

ċeaster, ċæster, 244

cēlan, 136

ċele, 236, 269

cēlnesse, 136

cēpte, 270

ċester, 257

ċiele, 75, 236

ċi̅e̅se, 241

ċietel, ċetel, 277

ċild, 7, 235

ċildru, 7

ċin(n), 76, 77

ċiriċe, 240

clǣnlīce, 271

clawu, 333

clēne, 236

cleopode, 79

cnāwan, 194, 274

cnear, 249, 281

cne̅o̅, 193

cōl, 136

coren, 269

costnian, 284

cran, 201

cugele, 244

cūþ, 153

cræft, 287

cwēn, 259, 319

cwĕne, 259

cwicu, cweocu,

c(w)ucu, 79

cȳ, 133

cyċene, 76, 244

cynn, 77, 233 n., 234

ċyrċe, 237, 238, 277

cyrfet, 242

 

dǣd, 192, 201, 202, 236, 263

dæg, 183, 227, 265

dægas, 80

dægum, 80

dagas, 265

dagian, 265, 284

deāþ, 265

de̅a̅(w), 286

dēd, 236, 263

dēman, 7, 10, 135

de̅o̅fol, 271

discipul, 247

dōgor, 183

dohter, 266

dōm, 7, 10

domne, 247

dragan, 267, 274, 333

dre̅a̅m, 262

drēfan, 148

drȳ, 239

dunn, 239

dūst, 68, 234

dȳstiġ, 234

 

Eādward, 270

e̅a̅ge, 228

e̅a̅gena, 289

eahta, 231

eald, āld, 45, 236

earm, 231

e̅a̅ðġete, 269

efel, 259, 319

efete, 267

ele, 234

eofor, 79

eolh, 231

eorþe, 231

etan, 203, 269

 

fæder, 134, 190, 200, 264, 269

fæsten, 284

fæstenian, 284

fæt, 78, 280

fatu, 78

featu, 78

fēdan, 148, 149

fēld, 319

feohtan, 231

fe̅o̅nd, 272

fēt, 203, 234

fetor, 79

fīc-be̅a̅m, 241

fīndan, 235

finugl, 244

fiscas, 133

flēmde, 271

fō, fēhþ, 234

fōda, 149, 263

forġeofan, 80

forfōr, 263

forloren, 269

forma, 194

forþ, 259

fōt, 142, 232, 234

fox, 234

fre̅o̅, 319

fre̅o̅nd, 272

freoðu-, 79

friðu, 79

from, 194

full, 149, 234

fulluht, 248

fulwian, 247

fulwiht, 248

furðor, 150

fylcian, 249

fyllan, 148, 149, 234

fȳlþ, 234, 271

fyrst, 150

fyxen, 234, 280

 

gāstlīc, 137

gāt, 228

ġe̅a̅r, 279

ġefan, 278, 279

ġelaþung, 248

ġelīce, 138

gelt, 237

ġenōge, 267

ġenōh, 258, 266

gēs, 8, 234

ġesīþ, 152

ġeslæġen, 234

ġest, 278

ġetan, 278

ġetri̅e̅we, 286

 

ġiċċan, 277

ġicel, 277

ġiefan, 54, 80, 258, 278, 279

ġielpan, 278

ġiest, 278

-ġietan, 278

ġif, 277

ġim-stān, 277

god, 234

gōd, 363

godspellere, 248

gold, 204, 234

gōs, 8, 152, 232, 233, 234

gōshafoc, 271

gyden, 234

gylden, 234

gylt, 237

 

hæfde, 259

hǣþ, 263, 319

hafoc, 267

hām, 213, 228, 260

hāmsocn, 283

hand, 154, 260

handġeweorc, 277

hē, 319

he̅a̅fod, 228

he̅a̅wan, 286

hēh, 266

heolstor, 79

heorot, 79

heorte, 199

hēr, 135, 319

hēran, 236, 257, 259

here, 233

hi̅e̅ran, hȳran, 236

hie, hira, heom, 287

hlāford, 259

hlahhan, 333

hnitu, 79

hopu, 269

hōs, 152

hryėġ, 238

hund, 153

hūs, 257

hūsl, 247

hūsl-þegn, 248

hūþ, 154

hwæl, 275

hwǣte, 275

hwǣr, 135

hȳd, 287

hynġr(i)an, 148

hȳran, 257, 280

 

lǣtan, 192

lāmb, 260

land, 228

leornung, 292

līc, 138

Lin(d)cylene, 244

lond, 228

lȳtle, 270

 

mæsse, 247

mǣþ, 227

mann, 228

māra, 262

market, 248

martyr, 247

mearm-stān, 244

medu, 157

meolc, 79

meriġ, 237

mētan, 148, 149

mete, 319

mētte, 270

midd, 203

mōdor, 134

mōna, 76, 229

mōnaþ, 271

monn, 228, 233

moraþ, 244

ġemōt, 149

mūþ, 152, 199

mynet, 241

myriġ, 237

 

nǣdl, 202

nǣp, 241

nama, 149, 228, 270, 291

nemnan, 148, 149

nest, 190, 203

nigun, 79

niman, 229, 287

nimanne, 80

to niomanne, 80

noma, 228

nōmon, 229

 

open, 323

ōra, 249, 281

ōþer, 152, 153

oxa, 204

 

pād, 201

pæll, 247

pāpa, 247

plōges, 267

plōh, 266

prāfost, 244

 

racu, 214

rāran, 262

reahte, 214

reċċean, 214

reġn, 265

regol, 79

reogol, 79

rīċe, 239

rūde, 244

 

sācerd, 247

sǣd, sēd, 236

sǣlan, 234

sæt, 186

sǣton, 190

sagu, 333

sāl, 234

sār, 260

sċ(e)amu, 213

sċe̅a̅p, sċēp, 133

sċearn, 247

sċe̅a̅wan, 286

sċeld, 236, 319

sċīeld, sċȳld, 236

sċieran, 247

se, seo, þæt, 293

sealm, 244

sēċan, 147

sēċ(e)an, 145, 146, 149

sēcst, sēcþ, 276

sēd, 319

sēman, 282

sendan, 154

senn, 237

seofon, 79

settan, 148

sicol, 79

sinu, 79

sīþ, 152, 154, 232

sittan, 190, 203

slēpte, 270

snetor, 237

snytor, 237

sōft, 152

sōfte, 232, 270

sōhte, 147, 149, 270

sōna, 323

sōt, 186, 190

sp(r)æc, 213

sprecol, 79

stān, 323

stānas, 289

stīgan, 201

strǣt, 241

sunu, 257

sunum (dat.), 290

swān, 283

Sweġen, 283

sweġer, 200

sweord, swurd, 237

sweotol, 79

swēte, 269

sword, 237

synn, 237

 

tæfl, 242

temian, 201

tēþ, 8

tōh, 274

tōþ, 8, 151, 153, 161, 163, 232

tre̅o̅we, 286

 

þæc, 234

þeċċean, 234

þenċan, 228

þe̅o̅f, 265

þe̅o̅n, 232

þōhte, 228, 231

þrǣll, 285

þrotu, 323

þūhte, 232

þynċean, 232

 

ūs, 232

ūtmest, 270

 

wæcen, 284

wæġn, 265

wǣpn, 227

wǣtan, 319

wæter, 264, 269

wāk, 286

wāld, 237, 266

weald, 236, 266

weġ, 258, 265

weodu, wudu, 78, 79

weorþ, wurþ, 237

werian, 148

wēron, 262

wētan, 319

wīflīc, 138

windan, 154

winnan, 154

wiodu, 78

wiorþeþ, 231

wīsdōm, 270

wiurþiþ, 231

worþ, 237

wudu, 78,79, 289

wulf, 290

wyrċan, 257

Wyrtġeorn, 244

 

yfel, 287

ynċe, 245, 246

yndse, 245

yntse, 245, 246

— — — —

Middle English.

appēren, 319

appi̅e̅ren, 319

ansuḗr, 262

auentūre, 263

aungel, 267, 333

babblen, 284

bē, 319

bēren, 259, 319

besēchen, 145

besēken, 145

bleu, 329

bliknen, 284

blōk, 286

blūd, 263

bond, 273

brēst, 272

brigge, 258

brofte, 274

brugge, 258

burien, 237

bustlen, 284

 

caf, 277

cause, 333

chappmenn, 270

chaunce, 123 n.

chaunge, 123 n.

chēfe, 319

chēke, 257

chele, 269

chester, 257

chetel, 277

chiefe, 319

chīld, 272

childre, 7

children, 7

chilldre, 272

chirche, 277

chōld, 266

chosenn, 269

clawe, 333

clennlike, 271

conclūd, 263

costnen, 285

court, 257

 

dai, 265, 266

dāme, 213, 261, 270

daunger, 333

daungerous, 267

dawen, 265

dawes, 265

dawnen, 284

day, 265

dayes, 265

deffles, 271

dei, 266

depthe, 270

deu, 286, 329

douhter, 266

drawen, 267, 274, 333

drēme, 262

dyath, 265

 

Edward, 270

ēȝ̇ene, 289

ei, 286

ere, 262

etenn, 269

eðgēte, 269

euel, 259

eute, 267

ēvel, 259, 319

 

fā̆der, 134, 264, 270, 271

faderr, 269

fāme, 261

fā̆ðer, 317

fēld, 268, 319

fēnd, 272

field, 268

fillthe, 271

findenn, 268

flemmde, 27

fless, flessch, 259

forfūre, 263

for-ȝete(n), yete(n), 277

fortṓne, 263

frē, 319

frēnd, 272

frendschipe, 272

fūde, 263

 

gǣfen, 278

gastli, 137

gastlich(e), 137

gate, 287

 

ȝelle(n), yelle(n), 277

ȝelpe(n), yelpe(n), 277

gentil, 123 n.

ȝēre, yēre, 277

ȝif, 277

ȝim, 277

ȝiuen, 258

 

god, 258, 323

goshauk, 271

gosling, 270

gōst, 137

gūd, 263

guod, 258

 

haggen, 286

hallghenn, 271

halwen, 271

hāme, 262

hand, 268

handfull, 235, 272

hanten, 335

hauk, 267

haunt, 333

haunten, 335

heeth, 260

 

heih, 266

hem, 287

hēren, 259, 280

hēþ, 319

hieren, 257

hir, 287

hit, 275

hōm, 213, 260

hōnd, 260, 268

hope, 269

hound, 268

hous, 257

huiren, 257, 280

hund, 268

huswīf, 271

hwīten, 284

 

icche(n), 277

icching, 277

i-cume, 277

if, 277

ikyl, 277

ille, 287

inogh, 58

inōuh, 266

inōwe, 267

itt, 275

 

jaundice, 333

joie, 123 n.

jointe, 123 n.

juge, 123 n.

jugement, 258

 

keppte, 270

kingene, 292

kingue, 258

kneu, 329

 

lāmb, 260

lambre, 272

lammbre, 260

lānd, 273

lātes, 261

lauerd, 259

laughen, 333

legges, 275

licóur}

lícour}, 123

līf, 259

little, 270

lōmb, 260, 272

lōnd, 273

long, 273

 

manér, 262

manéir, 262

mar, 262

me̅o̅c, 287

mēte, 319

mette, 270

monthe, 271

mōre, 261

 

nā̆me, 260, 270

neir, 262

 

ōld, 260, 323

ōpen, 323

ōre, 269

 

plesand, 293

plōuh, 266

plōwes, 267

 

quāle, 275

queen, 259

quēn, 319

quēne, 259

quēt, 275

 

rair, 262

rā̆ðer, 75, 317

rein, 266

rūde, 263

 

sāwe, 333

sayand, 293

scatteren, 283

schāme, 213

schēld, 319

schip, 259

sēchen, 145, 146

sēken, 145, 147

sēkst, 276

sēkþ, 276

seldcēne, 257

sēmelich, 282

sēmen, 282

sēmli, 282

serrfenn, 256

serruen, 256

shatteren, 283

skill, 287

skinn, 287

sleppte, 270

soffte, 270

sohhte, 270

sone, 257

sōne, 263

sōr, 260

ssip, 259

stōn, 291, 323

stōnes, 289

stoon, 260

strang, 268

strōng, 268, 273

swēte, 269

syngand, 293

 

tāhte, 336

takenn, 287

þe, þeo, þet, 293

thinken, 259

þrōte, 323

þyef, 265

til, 287

 

uader, 280

uorþ, 259

utmōst, 270

 

vertúe}

vértue}, 123

vorlōre(n), 269

 

wǣld, 266

wain, 265

war, 262

wāt, 261

wā̆ter, 264, 271

weȝ̇, 258

wei, 265

were, 262

wigt, 285

wimman, 271

wissdōm, 270

wode, 289

wōk, 286

wurchen, 257

 

ylde, 277

ym-stōn, 277

 

zēchen, 280

— — — —

English.

ale, 230

all, 267, 312, 333

Alphege, 377

alter [ɔ̄ltə], 360

Alvescot [ɔ̄lskət, etc.], 379

among [əmaŋ, netc.], 361

Ardingley, 378

ass, 229

Atterbury, 293

aught, 335

aunt, 267

 

ball, 334

band, 273

bat, 38

bath, 317

batten, 284

bawl, 267

to bear, 214

bee, 60

begin, 278, 279

beseech, 145, 147, 276

beseek (dial.), 145

bet, 38, 39, 43

better, 284, 318

bird, 38, 53

bishopric, 239

bit, 38, 40, 43

bite, 49

bitterly, 131

bleak, 286

blood, 307, 325, 327, 361

blue, 329, 330

boil, 323

bold, 237

bond, 273

book, 133, 324, 325, 327, 361

book-case, 48

boot, 38, 42, 53

boots, 360

bought, 337

boys, 130

brandy pawnee, 74

bread, 321

break, 321

breath, 6, 318

breathe, 6, 318

bridge, 238

broft (dial.), 274

broil, 322

broken, 213

brother, 130, 134, 327

brought, 336

bruise, 264, 330

buck, 325

buik (Sc.), 53

bull, 326

bury, 237

bush, 41

but, 53, 314, 322, 325, 326

butcher, 41

 

Cabul, 74

calf, 335

call, 333, 334

calm, 335

came, 131

can, 313

cane, 314

car, 35

cast, 313

cat, 53, 130

Cawnpore, 74

chance, 334

charmed, 131

cheese, 241

child, 7, 235, 272

children, 7, 131, 235, 272

chill, 75, 136, 236

chin, 76, 77

chivalry, 364

church, 237

Cirencester, 378

Clark, 318

clerk, 74, 317, 318, 361

cletch (dial.), 277

clutch (dial.), 277

coffee, 360

cold, 75, 136, 237

contradict, 127

cool, 136

to cool, 136

coolness, 136

cough [kɔ̄f, etc.] 360

courtesy [kɔ̄təsi, etc.], 360

cows, 133

cure, 330

cut, 327

 

dag (dial.), 285

daggle (dial.), 284

dame, 213, 270

dams, 133

dance, 334, 360

dangle, 284

daughter, 336

daunt, 334

dead, 321

deed, 236, 263

deem, 7, 135, 137

Derby, 317, 318, 361

desire, 366

dew, 286, 329

disaster, 127

ditch, 276

dog, 130

-dom, 135

doom, 7, 135, 137

draught, 325

draw, 274

druid, 239

duke, 330

dust, 68

 

eat, 269

eave (dial.), 278

egg, 286

enow, 267

envelope, 123

 

face, 259, 317

falcon, 364

falconry, 364

fall, 333, 334

far, 317

father, 38, 39, 42, 53, 54, 74, 130, 134, 269, 360

feet, 137

female, 134

few, 330

field, 268

fiend, 272

find, 268

fine, 322

fish, 113, 133

fishes, 134

flaunt, 334

fleck (dial.), 276

flick (dial.), 276

flesh, 133, 272

fling, 131, 132

flitch, 276

flock, 133

flood, 325

flung, 131, 132

food, 133

fool, 324, 328

foot, 137, 324, 327

forehead [fɔrid, etc.], 365, 378

forlorn, 135

forsworn, 136

forward, 378

friend, 272, 318, 319

frighten, 284

full, 325, 326, 327, 328

 

gall, 312

gave, 131

geese, 8

get, 278

ghastly, 137

ghostly, 137

gif (dial.), 278

gift, 278

gilpie (dial.), 278

give, 278, 279

gladden, 284

good, 31, 35, 324 325

goose, 8

grant, 334

great, 321

Greenwich [grinidž, etc.], 365

ground, 73

guest, 258, 278

guide, 322

gut, 325

 

hale, 311

hall, 334

hand, 272

handiwork, 277

hang, 273

hardly, 131

hat, 311

haw, 334

haunch, 267, 334

haunt, 267, 334

have, 46

head, 53, 321

hear, 167, 236

heard, 167, 318

heart, 317, 318

hearth, 317, 318

heath, 263

(h)eave (dial.), 278

Helingly, 378

herd, 133

here, there, everywhere, 131, 134

here, 131, 135

hew, 286

hit (Sc.), 275

horse, hoarse, 16

hot, 53, 54

hound, 268

house, 73

housen (dial.), 292

houses, 131

humorous, [jūmərəs, etc.], 360

humour, 127

hundred, 112

 

icicle, 277

ill, 131

inch, 245

itch, 277

 

jaundice, 267, 334

jaunt, 334

jest, 275

join, 322, 323

joint, 322

joy, 275

judge, 275

 

ken, 314

kernel, colonel, 16

kettle, 277

Kilconquahar, 378

kin, 77

king, 35, 134

kirk (Sc.), 277

knave, 337

knew, 329

Knowsley, 379

 

lamb, 235, 272, 365

lambs, 133

lance, 334

land, 273

laughter, 335, 337

Launcelot, 334

launch, 334

laundry, 267, 334

lawful, 334

learn [lʌ̄n, etc.], 318, 361

leave, 320

light, 366

-like, 138

Lincoln, 244

line, 322

literature [litərətʃə, etc.], 365

loch (Sc.), 32, 35

long, 229, 273

look, 325

loose, 324

lorn, 136

lose, 135, 136

love, 365

luck, 325

lust, 325

 

man, 132

manlike, 138

manly, 138

maw, 334

meat, 320

men, 132, 318

merry, 237

mice, 131

midge (dial.), 276

Midhurst, 378

mirth, 237

moon, 76

mother, 130, 134, 327

mud, 325, 326

muse, 330

 

name, 260, 270, 317

nim (dial.), 287

nonce, for the, 293

nut, 327

 

of, 214, 215

off, 214, 215

oil, 323

old, 45, 237

 

pail, 332

Parma—Palmer, 16

pass, 313

past, 314

phonograph, 127

placed, 131

pleasure, 35

plough, 266

poignant [poinənt, etc.], 365

priest, 272

prime, 366

primrosen (dial.), 292

psalm, 335

pull, 325, 326, 327

punt, 326

pure [pjɔ̄, etc.], 360

put, 53, 326, (Sc.) 53

 

quality, 308

qualm, 335

quantity, 308

queen (Sc.), 134

 

railway, 360

rang, 131

rather, 74, 317

red, 321

redden, 284

rhyme, 366

ridge, 238, 276

righteous [raitʃəs, etc.], 365

ring, 131

root, 323

Rudge, 238

rule, 330

rang, 131

 

saint, St., 215

Sanders, 334

sang, 131, 167, 273

salt, 267

Saunders, 334

saw, 53, 60

scag (dial.), 286

scant, 285

scug (dial.), 286

sea, 31

sedge, 276

see, 38, 39, 53

seech (dial.), 145

seed, 236

seek, 145, 147, 276

seemly, 282

seg (dial.), 276

sent, 131

servant, 318

set, 318

shame, 213

sheep, 133

shemale (pop.), 134

shield, 236

ship, 35

shoe, 324

sing, 29, 33, 35, 131, 167

sit, 39

small, 312

sought, 147

southern [saðən, etc.], 365

spoken, 213

spoon [spūn, etc.], 323, 327, 361

star, 317

steak, 321

stick, 132

straw, 334

street, 241

strong, 229, 273

stuck, 132

stupid, 330

suffer, 325

sung, 131, 167

 

tail, 332

tane and the tither, the (Sc.), 294

taught, 335

taunt, 334

teeth, 8

telegraph, 127

telephone, 127

the, 112

their, they, them, 287

there, 135

think, 29, 32

this, 29

thoft (dial.), 274

thought, 336

threw, 330

thunder, 325

til (dial.), 287

time, 322

told, 131

tooth, 8, 151, 161

t’other, the (obs.), 294

tough, 274

trees, 130

trig (dial.), 286

true, 286

Tuesday, 330

 

until, 287

 

vase, 74

vat, 280

vaunt, 334

virtue [vʌ̄tjū, etc.], 361

vixen, 280

 

Wadhurst, 378

wall, 334

wane, 314

was, 314

water, 269

weak, 286

weald, 266

weet (Sc.), 319

well, 131

went, 131

wet, 319

where, 135

wife, 322

wifelike, 138

wifely, 138

wight (dial.), 285

wine, 322

winefat, 280

wold, 237, 266

womanly, 138

wood, 324

wrath [rɔ̄þ, etc.], 361

write, 337

wrote, 131

 

yclept, 277

yeave (dial. obs.), 278

yeavey (dial. obs.), 278

— — — —

Old Saxon.

ahto, 231

akkar, 227

 

bindan, 203

 

crano, 201

 

ertha, 231

etan, 203

 

fallan, 198

fūliþa, 234

 

gast, 202

 

jung, 153

 

māno, 229

middi, 203

 

rīki, 239

 

sibun, 199

sittian, 203

sōkian, 147

strāta, 241

 

thīhan, 232

 

werk, 203

— — — —

Old Frisian.

jung, 153

— — — —

Dutch.

dag, 167

drie, 167

 

een, 167

 

hoorde, 167

hooren, 167

 

komme(n), 167

 

moeder, 167

 

twee, 167

 

vier, 167

vijf, 167

 

zingen, 167

zong, 167

ge-zongen, 167

 

Old High German.

acchar, 227

ahto, 231

andar, 152

arm, 231

 

bintan, 154

bītan, 202

 

chāsi, 241

chirihha, 240

cheisar, 241

chund, 153

churbizz, 242

 

dãhta, 228

denken, dâchta, 231

dīhan, 232

dunst, 234

 

erda, 229

ewist, 193

 

fallan, 198

fehtan, 231

fuoz, 142

 

gans, 152, 232

gast, 202

geiz, 228

gisindo, 152

gitriuwi, 286

 

hansa, 152

hant, 154

heim, 228

heri-hunda, 154

houbit, 228

houwan, 286

hunt, 153

 

jung, 153

 

kalt, 231

kocchōn, 158

kunst, 194

 

mād, 227

Maginza, 158

māno, 229

metu, 157

mund, 152

mūs, 112

 

nādala, 202

nāmum, 229

nasa, 191

nest, 203

 

ouga, 228

 

rīhhi, 239

 

samfto, 152, 232

sind, 152, 232

sizzen, 203

strāzza, 241

suohhan, 147

 

tac, 227

tāt, 202

tou, 286

tuomian, 10

 

uns, 232

 

vinnan, 154

vintan, 154

vruo, 194

 

wāfan, 227

Wasconowalt, 158

werc, 203

 

zabal, 242

zand, 151, 153, 161, 163, 232

— — — —

Middle High German.

elch, 231

— — — —

German.

alt, 45

 

blume, 53

 

drei, 167

 

ei, 286

ein, 167

 

fünf, 167

 

genie [ʃϵnī́], 123

 

hat, 53

hören, hörte, 167

 

käse, 241

kommen, 167

 

lohn, 53

 

maus, 112

mutter, 167

 

reich, 239

 

sang, 167

schauen, 286

singen, 167

sorge, 35

stock, 53

ge-sungen, 167

 

tag, 167

träue, 286

 

vaterland, 122

vier, 167

 

zwei, 167

— — — —

Lithuanian.

avimis, 290

avis, 157, 158

 

bendras, 156, 162, 163, 203

 

dantis, 155, 161

 

esmi, 157

ẽsti, 157

 

keturi, 160

 

medùs, 157

 

naktis, 157, 158

 

-patis, 157

pílnas, 194

pirmas, 194

 

sẽnas, 157

szimtas, 112, 162, 163

 

žinóti, 194

— — — —

Old Slavonic.

dy-mŭ, 68

 

nosti, 158

 

ovitsa, 158

 

sedeti, 190

— — — —

Russian.

[ɫoʃad], 35

 

otíchestvo, 122

— — — —

Finnish.

kulta, 204


LIST OF AUTHORITIES REFERRED TO

[This list does not include the monographs, etc., enumerated in the lists in Chapters XII. and XIV.]

Bechtel: Hauptprobleme der indogerm. Lautlehre seit Schleicher, 1892.

Bell, Melville: Visible Speech.

Björkman: Scandinavian Loan-Words in Middle English, Part I., Halle, 1900.

Bopp, F.: Vergleichende Grammatik (3rd ed.); Vocalismus, 1836.

Brate, E.: Nordische Lehnwörter im Ormulum, Beitr. X. (1884), 1-80.

Bremer, O.: Ethnographie der germ. Stamme,² 1900.

Brugmann: Griechische Grammatik,³ 1900; Grundriss der Vergleichenden Grammatik der Indogermanischen Sprachen (2nd ed.), Bd. I. (Lautlehre), 1897; Kurze Vergleichende Grammatik der Indogermanischen Sprachen, Bd. I. (Lautlehre), 1902; Zum heutigen Stand der Sprachwissenschaft, 1885; Zur Frage nach den Verwandtschaftsverhältnissen der Idg. Spr. (in Techmer’s Zeitschrift für allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft I.).

Bülbring, K. D.: Altenglisches Elementarbuch. I. Lautlehre, Heidelberg, 1902.

Darmsteter: La Vie des Mots, 1887.

Dibelius: John Capgrave und die englische Schriftsprache, Anglia XXIII., p. 152, etc.

Dieter: Laut- und Formenlehre d. altgermanischen Dialekte, vol. i., 1898.

Ellis, A. J.: Early English Pronunciation, Parts I.-IV., 1869-1874.

Greenough and Kittredge: Words and their Ways in English Speech, 1902.

Grimm: Deutsche Grammatik, vols. i.-iv., 1822-1837.

Hargreaves: The Addlington Dialect. Heidelberg, 1904.

Hirt: d. Idg. Ablaut, 1900; Griechische Grammatik, 1902; Verwandtschaftsverhältnisse der Indogermanen, in Indogermanische Forschungen IV., pp. 36-45; Urheimat der Indogerm., in Indog. Forsch. I.

Jespersen: Lehrbuch der Phonetik, 1904.

Koeppel: Spelling Pronunciations: Bemerkungen über den Einfluss des Schriftbildes auf den Laut im Englischen. Strassbourg, 1901. (Quellen u. Forschungen, Bd. 89).

Kaluza, M.: Historische Grammatik der Englischen Sprache, vol. i. Berlin, 1900. Vol. ii., 1901.

Kluge, Fr.: Geschichte der Englischen Sprache, in Paul’s Grundriss; Vorgeschichte der germanischen Sprachen, in Paul’s Grundriss.²

Kretschmer: Einleitung in die Gesch. d. griech. Sprache, 1896.

Leskien: Deklination im Slavisch und Deutsch, 1876.

Loth: Angelsachsen und Romanen, in Englische Studien XIX; Les Mots Latins dans les Langues Brittoniques, 1892.

MacGillivray, H. S.: The Influence of Christianity on the Vocabulary of Old English. Part I. Halle, 1902.

Morris: Historical Outlines of English Accidence, edited by Bradley.

Morris and Skeat: Specimens of Early English.

Morsbach, L.: Anglia Beiblatt VII; Mittelenglische Grammatik, 1 Theil. Halle, 1896; Über den Ursprung der neuenglischen Schriftsprache, Heilbronn, 1888.

Napier, A.: Notes on Orthography of the Ormulum, Academy, 1890; and in History of the Holy Rood-tree, E. E. T. S., 1894, p. 71.

Noreen, A.: Urgermanische Lautlehre, 1894.

Ostermann: Lautlehre Ancren Riwle, Bonner Beiträge, 1905.

Osthoff: Das physiologische und das psychologische Moment in der sprachlichen Formenbildung, 1879; Schriftsprache und Volksmundart, Berlin, 1883.

Osthoff and Brugmann: Morphologische Untersuchungen, Vol. I., 1878.

Passy, Paul: Changements Phonétiques du Langage. Paris, 1891.

Paul: Principien der Sprachgeschichte.

Pogatscher: Zur Lautlehre der griech., lat. und roman. Lehnwörter im altenglischen (Q. F. 64). Strassburg, 1888.

Rippmann, W.: The Sounds of Spoken English. London, 1906.

Scherer: Geschichte d. deutschen Sprache, 1868.

Schleicher: Compendium,² 1866; Deutsche Sprache,² 1869.

Schmidt, Johann: Verwandtschaftsverhältnisse der Idg. Sprachen, 1872.

Schrader: Urheimat der Indogermanen, in Reallexikon der Indogerm. Altertumskunde 1901; Sprachvergleichung und Urgeschichte, 1890.

Siebs: Zur Geschichte der engl.-friesisch. Sprache, 1889.

Sievers, E.: Angelsächsische Grammatik,³ Halle, 1898; Phonetik, 4th ed.

Skeat: Concise Etymological Dictionary of the English Language, 1901; Principles of English Etymology.

Smith, Gregory: Specimens of Middle Scots.

Streitberg, W., Indogerm, Forschungen, iii. 305, etc.; Urgermanische Grammatik.

Strong, Logemann and Wheeler: History of Language, 1891.

Sweet, Henry: Cura Pastoralis, Introduction; History of English Sounds, Oxford, 1888; History of Language, 1900; New English Grammar, Part I., Oxford, 1892; Primer of Phonetics (2nd ed.), Oxford, 1902; Primer of Historical English Grammar; Primer of Spoken English (3rd ed.), Oxford, 1900; Shorter English Historical Grammar; Words, Logic, and Grammar, Trans. Phil. Soc., 1875-76.

Taylor, Isaac: The Origin of the Aryans, 1890.

Ten Brink: Chaucers Sprache und Verskunst, Leipzig, 1899.

Wechssler: Gibt es Lautgesetze? 1900.

Whitney: Language and its Study, 1875; Life and Growth of Language, 1886.

Wright, Joseph: English Dialect Grammar, 1905; Grammar of the Dialect of Windhill, E.D.S., 1892.

Wyld: History of O.E. ġ in the Middle and Modern English Dialects, Otia Merseiana, vol. ii.; Engl. Studien, XXVIII., p. 393, etc.; Otia Merseiana, IV., p. 75, etc.

THE END

PRINTED IN GREAT BRITAIN BY BILLING AND SONS, LTD., GUILDFORD AND ESHER.

Transcriber’s Notes

This book employs a wide range of characters, some with various diacritic marks. Some of the characters were chosen by the author to mimic the appearance of handwritten characters in manuscripts that pre-date moveable type. Further, the book was printed before Unicode and the International Phonetic Alphabet were established to standardise the naming and appearance of printable characters. Consequently there are challenges in transferring such a book to a digital format.

A character resembling both ɛ ‘Latin small letter open e’ and ε ‘Greek small letter epsilon’ occurs frequently. When the character is clearly part of a Greek word then the second character is used. Otherwise the first is used.

In the text the character ϵ ‘Greek lunate epsilon’ is not used in a Greek word, but as representing a sound. However it occurred frequently in the Greek words in the Word Index. In the text it may be equivalent to ɛ ‘Latin small letter open e’, used because of insufficient supply of ɛ type, or to represent a distinct vocal sound. In the Word Index all of the epsilon characters in Greek words are given as the Greek epsilon ε.

The characters þ ‘thorn’, ð ‘eth’ and ȝ ‘yogh’ did not occur in an italic form in this book, even when a part of a word otherwise entirely in italics. It was presumed that this arose from the typesetter lacking an italic version of these characters. In all such cases, þ, ð and ȝ have been formatted as þ, ð and ȝ respectively.

The italic versions of the diphthongs æ and œ, æ and œ respectively, were very difficult to distinguish in the original. It was frequently the case that the correct character could not be inferred from context. Consequently these two characters may be erroneously interchanged on occasion.

The footnotes have been renumbered sequentially throughout the entire book.

Minor typographic errors, such as missing or misordered letters, were corrected silently.

[The end of The Historical Study of the Mother Tongue by Henry Cecil Wyld]